Predictors of Sexual Recidivism: An Updated Meta-Analysis 2004-02
R. Karl Hanson and Kelly Morton-Bourgon
Abstract
This quantitative review examined the research evidence concerning recidivism risk factors for sexual offenders. A total of 95 different studies were examined, involving more than 31,000 sexual offenders and close to 2000 recidivism predictions. The results confirmed deviant sexual interests and antisocial orientation as important predictors of sexual recidivism. Antisocial orientation (e.g., unstable lifestyle, history of rule violation) was a particularly important predictor of violent non-sexual recidivism and general recidivism. The study also identified a number of new predictor variables, some of which have the potential of being useful targets for intervention (e.g., sexual preoccupations, conflicts in intimate relationships, emotional identification with children, hostility). Actuarial risk instruments were consistently more accurate than unguided clinical opinion in predicting sexual, violent non-sexual and general recidivism. For the prediction of sexual recidivism, there were no significant differences in the predictive accuracy of the various actuarial measures (e.g., SORAG, Static-99). Actuarial measures designed to predict general (any) criminal recidivism were strong predictors of general recidivism among sexual offenders.
Predictors of Sexual Recidivism: An Updated Meta-Analysis
Sex offences are among the crimes that invoke the most public concern. Consequently, it is not surprising that exceptional policies have been directed towards individuals who have committed such offences, including long-term supervision, treatment, civil commitment, community notification and public registries. The effective administration of such policies require evaluation of the offenders' recidivism risk. Approximately 1%- 2% of the adult male population will eventually be convicted of a sexual offence (California Office of the Attorney General, 2003; Marshall, 1997). Not all sexual offenders, however, are equally likely to reoffend. The observed sexual recidivism rate among typical groups of sexual offenders is in the range of 10%-15% after 5 years (Hanson & Bussière, 1998); there are, however, identifiable subgroups whose observed recidivism rates are much higher (Harris et al., 2003). Interventions directed towards the highest risk offenders are most likely to contribute to public safety.
Knowledge concerning sexual offender recidivism risk has advanced considerably during the past 10 years. Prior to the 1990s, evaluators had little empirical guidance concerning the factors that were, or were not, related to recidivism risk. There is now a general consensus that sexual recidivism is associated with at least two broad factors: a) deviant sexual interests, and b) antisocial orientation/lifestyle instability (Hanson & Bussière, 1998; Quinsey, Lalumière, Rice & Harris, 1995; Roberts, Doren & Thornton, 2002). Although all sexual offending is socially deviant, not all offenders have an enduring interest in sexual acts that are illegal (e.g., children, rape) or highly unusual (e.g., fetishism, auto-erotic asphyxia). Sexual recidivism increases when such deviant interests are present, as indicated by self-report, offence history, or specialized testing (Hanson & Bussière, 1998).
Individuals with deviant sexual interest will not commit sexual crimes, however, unless they are willing to hurt others to obtain their goals, can convince themselves that they are not harming their victims, or feel unable to stop themselves. Sexual crimes, like other crimes, are often associated with an antisocial orientation and lifestyle instability (crime-prone personality). Such individuals tend to engage in a range of impulsive, reckless behaviour, such as excessive drinking, frequent moves, fights, and unsafe work practices (Caspi et al., 1995; Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). Another element of the crime-prone personality is a hostile, resentful attitude (Andrews & Bonta, 2003; Caspi et al., 1995). Rapists are more likely to have an antisocial orientation than are child molesters (Firestone, Bradford, Greenberg & Serran, 2000; see review by West, 1983), but indicators of hostility and lifestyle instability are associated with sexual recidivism in both groups (Prentky, Knight, Lee & Cerce, 1995; Rice, Quinsey & Harris, 1991).
Despite the progress in recent years, much remains to be known about how to identify and manage potential recidivists. Sexual offenders have many life problems, not all of which are related to offending. In order to reduce recidivism risk, interventions need to address the enduring characteristics associated with recidivism risk, which have been referred to as “criminogenic needs” (Andrews & Bonta, 2003), “stable dynamic risk factors” (Hanson & Harris, 2000b) or “causal psychological risk factors” (Beech & Ward, in press). One route to improving sexual offender risk assessment is to improve our understanding of the processes motivating this behaviour.
A model of sex offence risk
Contemporary theories posit that a variety of factors are associated with sexual offending (Ward & Siegert, 2002; Knight & Sims-Knight, 2003; Malamuth, 2003). These models suggest that the breeding ground for sexual offending is an adverse family environment, characterized by various forms of abuse and neglect. The lack of nurturance and guidance leads to problems in social functioning, such as mistrust, hostility, and insecure attachment, which, in turn are associated with social rejection, loneliness, negative peer associations and delinquent behaviour. The form of sexuality that develops in the context of pervasive intimacy deficits is likely to be impersonal and selfish, and may even be adversarial. Further contributing to the risk of sexual offending are beliefs that permit non-consenting sex. Attitudes allowing non-consenting sex can develop through the individuals' effort to understand their own experiences, and by adopting the attitudes of their significant others (friends, family, abusers).
Such a model suggests that apart from sexual deviancy and lifestyle instability, there may be three additional characteristics of persistent sexual offenders: a) negative family background, b) problems with friends and lovers, and c) attitudes tolerant of sexual assault. Examination of the most commonly used structured rating scales for sexual offenders indicate that these content areas have considerable credibility among those conducting sexual offender evaluations (Beech, Fisher & Thornton, 2003). The Sexual Violence Risk–20 (SVR-20, Boer, Hart, Kropp & Webster, 1997), the Sex Offender Need Assessment Rating (SONAR, Hanson & Harris, 2001), and the Structured Risk Assessment (SRA, Thornton, 2002a) all have sections addressing sexual interests, pro-offending attitudes, intimacy deficits, and general problems with impulsivity and criminality. Of the above rating scales, only the SVR-20 contains an item addressing negative development history, and only one aspect of it - sexually abused as child.
The research evidence concerning some of these content areas, however, is surprisingly weak. For example, Hanson and Bussière's (1996, 1998) meta-analytic review of sex offender recidivism studies found that the average correlation between sexual recidivism and being sexual abused as a child was r = -.01 (based on 5 studies). For deviant sexual attitudes, the studies found different results, and the average relationship with sexual recidivism was small (average r = .09, 4 studies). Apart from marital status, indicators of intimacy problems (e.g., loneliness, negative peer associations) were rarely examined in the recidivism studies available in 1996, the cut-off period for Hanson and Bussière's (1998) review. It is important to remember that the factors associated with becoming a sex offender (initiation factors) need not be the same factors that predict recidivism.
Research evidence is important because some highly plausible factors may not be related to recidivism. For example, three factors commonly considered in risk assessments are a) the seriousness of the index offence (e.g., victim injury, intercourse), b) internalizing psychological problems (e.g., low self-esteem, depression), and c) clinical presentation (e.g., denial, lack of victim empathy, low motivation for treatment). None of these features, however, were related to sexual recidivism risk in Hanson and Bussière's (1998) review. Although such counter-intuitive results require careful scrutiny before they result in changes in applied risk assessment (Lund, 2000), it is nevertheless difficult for evaluators to justify the continued use of risk factors that have no relationship to the outcome they are asked to predict.
Combining risk factors
Even when evaluators consider valid risk factors, it is not clear how the risk factors should be combined into an overall evaluation. Until recently, almost all sex offender risk assessments were based on unguided clinical judgement. In these assessments, experts used their experience and understanding of a specific case to make predictions about future behaviour. Given that the predictive accuracy of unguided clinical assessments are typically only slightly above chance levels (Hanson & Bussière, 1998), attention has shifted to empirically based methods of risk assessment. In the empirically-guided approach, evaluators are given a list of researched-based risk factors to consider, but the method of combining the factors into an overall evaluation is not specified (e.g., SVR-20; Boer et al., 1997).
In contrast, actuarial approaches not only specify the items to consider, but they provide explicit directions on how to combine the items into an overall risk score (e.g., Violence Risk Appraisal Guide [VRAG], Quinsey, Rice, Harris & Cormier, 1998). Similarly, the adjusted actuarial approach begins with the predictions generated by an actuarial scheme, but then asks whether the actuarial predictions fairly represent the risk of the specific offender after considering characteristics external to the actuarial scheme (e.g., stated intentions to reoffend, debilitating health problems) (Webster, Harris, Rice, Cormier & Quinsey, 1994).
Given that actuarial measures have a known degree of predictive accuracy (in the moderate range), and can be reliably scored from commonly available information (e.g., demographic and criminal history), they have been rapidly adopted by evaluators and decision-makers. Actuarial measures have been recommended as a component of best practices (Beech et al., 2003) and many routine decisions in the criminal justice system (e.g., intensity of treatment, community notification) are now based on actuarial measures.
The actuarial measures most commonly used with sexual offenders are the Minnesota Sex Offender Screening Tool – Revised (MnSOST-R; Epperson, Kaul, & Hesselton, 1998); the Violence Risk Appraisal Guide (VRAG) and the Sex Offender Risk Appraisal Guide (SORAG; Quinsey et al., 1998); the Rapid Risk Assessment for Sex Offence Recidivism (RRASOR; Hanson, 1997) and Static-99 (Hanson & Thornton, 2000). All of these measures contain primarily static, historical factors, such as prior sex offences, prior other offences, and the characteristics of the victims. The VRAG and SORAG are most heavily weighted with psychopathy, early childhood behaviour problems, and other indicators of antisocial orientation. The four items of the RRASOR (age less than 25, prior sex offences, male victims, unrelated victims) are fully included in the 10-item Static-99 (the additional items primarily address non-sexual criminal history).
Considerable research has been conducted in recent years examining the predictive accuracy of actuarial measures. Replication research is important if such instruments are to be used in applied decision making (Campbell, 2000). The existence of multiple instruments has also motivated research comparing the predictive accuracy of the different measures in various samples (e.g., Barbaree, Seto, Langton, & Peacock, 2001; Harris et al., 2003; Nunes, Firestone, Bradford, Greenberg & Broom, 2002; Sjöstedt & Långström, 2002). The predictive accuracies of the measures are typically in the moderate range, and no single measure has been consistently superior across samples. Further analyses are required to determine whether the variability in the predictive accuracy of the measures is more than would be expected by chance.
The need for an updated review
Hanson and Bussière's (1996, 1998) meta-analysis made an important contribution to sexual offender risk assessment by summarizing the available evidence concerning recidivism risk factors. The results of a single study can be interesting, but decision-makers can have increased confidence in research results when the same relationship is found in many studies. Meta-analyses, like any other research, need to be scrutinized and revised in light of new evidence.
Some of Hanson and Bussière's (1998) findings were based on sufficiently large numbers of offenders from diverse settings that further research is unlikely to change the results. For example, the positive correlation of r = .19 between prior sex offences and future sex offences was based on 11,294 offenders from 29 different samples (95% confidence interval of .17 - .21). Other factors with strong empirical support included deviant sexual preferences, antisocial personality, diverse sex crimes, never married, victim characteristics (male, unrelated, strangers) and failure to complete treatment (for additional studies on treatment drop-out, see Hanson et al., 2002). Some of Hanson and Bussière's findings, however, were still provisional, being based on small samples sizes (e.g., negative relationship with mother, n = 378, 3 studies), or studies that found conflicting results (e.g., employment instability, Q = 106.6, p < .001, 6 studies).
The purpose of the current study was to update Hanson and Bussière's (1998) meta-analysis in light of the ongoing research on sexual offender risk assessment. Rather than repeat all the variables from Hanson and Bussière (1998), the current study considered only findings that a) were considered important to applied risk assessment, and b) were weak or controversial (e.g., denial, victim damage) in the earlier review. Whereas Hanson and Bussière (1998) examined primarily static, historical factors, the focus of the current study was on potentially changeable (dynamic) risk factors. It was hoped that there were sufficient studies since the cut-off date of the earlier review (1996) that conclusions could now be drawn about dynamic risk factors (i.e., the risk factors needed for identifying treatment targets and evaluating change). The recent research on actuarial risk assessment with sexual offenders also provided an opportunity to compare different approaches to risk assessment (unstructured clinical, empirically guided, pure actuarial), and to compare the predictive validity of the various actuarial measures.
For the general public, sexual recidivism is highly worrisome, and some risk evaluations are solely concerned with this type of recidivism. It is important, however, to consider other types of recidivism when assessing the risk presented by sexual offenders. Sexual offenders are more likely to reoffend with a non-sexual offence than a sexual offence (Hanson & Bussière, 1998), and policies aimed at public protection should also be concerned with the likelihood of any form of serious recidivism, not just sexual recidivism. In this respect, an important question is whether the predictors of sexual recidivism are substantially different from the predictors of non-sexual recidivism. Consequently, the review examined four different types of recidivism: a) sexual recidivism, b) violent non-sexual recidivism, c) any violent recidivism (sexual and non-sexual), and d) any recidivism (violent and non-violent).
Method
Sample
Computer searches of PsycLIT, the National Criminal Justice Reference Service (USA), and the Library of the Solicitor General of Canada were conducted using the following key terms: child molester, exhibitionism, exhibitionist, failure, frotteur, incest, indecent exposure, paraphilias (c), pedophile, pedophilia, predict (ion), rape, rapist, recidivate, recidivism, recidivist, relapse, reoffend, reoffense, sex (ual) offender, sexual assault, sexual deviant. Articles were also found by reviewing the reference lists of empirical studies and previous reviews, and by scouring recent issues of relevant journals (e.g., Criminal Justice and Behavior, Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment). Finally, letters were sent to 34 established researchers in the field of sexual offender recidivism requesting overlooked or as-yet unpublished articles or data.
To be included in the present meta-analysis, the study had to involve an identifiable sample of sex offenders, either adults or adolescents. Studies where the index offence was not a sexual offence were excluded, even if some of the subjects had committed a sexual offence in the past. Studies had to examine sexual, violent or any recidivism that occurred after a specified period of time (e.g., after release from a correctional institution). Excluded were retrospective studies that only included the offenders' criminal history, prior to the index offence. Also excluded were studies that used broad definitions of failure, including, for example, treatment drop-out along with criminal recidivism (Maletzky, 1993). All studies had to provide information concerning one of the characteristics targeted in this review (a complete list is available upon request). Finally, studies needed to provide sufficient statistical information: a) sample size, b) the rate of recidivism, and c) sufficient information to estimate the effect size d (see Appendix). In order to reduce the variability associated with very small sample sizes, at least 5 subjects for all marginal totals were required for dichotomous variables.
As of January, 2003, our search yielded 153 usable documents (e.g., published articles, books, government reports, unpublished program evaluations, conference presentations). In 20 cases, the analyses were based on raw data or analyses obtained directly from the original researchers. When the same data set was reported in several articles, all the results from these articles were considered to come from the same study. Consequently, the 153 documents represented 95 different studies (country of origin: 42 United States, 30 Canada, 13 United Kingdom, 3 Austria, 2 Sweden, 2 Australia, and one each from France, Netherlands and Denmark; 49 (51.6%) unpublished; produced between 1943 and 2003, with a median of 1997; average sample size of 332, median of 167, range of 10 to 3185). Thirty-two of the samples were the same as those included in Hanson and Bussière's (1998) review, 12 studies contained updated information (e.g., longer follow-up periods, new analyses), and 51 studies were new.
Most of the studies examined mixed groups of adult sexual offenders (84 mixed offence types, 8 child molesters, 2 rapists, 1 exhibitionists; 79 predominantly adults, 16 adolescents). Most of the offenders were released from institutions (52 institution only, 17 community only, 24 institution and community, and 2 unknown). Thirty-seven samples came from treatment programs, and 56 samples contained a mixture of treated and untreated offenders, or the extent of treatment was unknown. When demographic information was presented, the offenders were predominantly Caucasian (44 of 48 studies). The sexual offenders were almost all males, with the exception of one study of female sexual offenders (Williams & Nicholaichuk, 2001).
The most common sources of recidivism information were national criminal justice records (50), provincial or state records (39), records from treatment programs (21) and self-reports (14). Other sources (e.g., child protection records, parole files) were used in 21 studies. In 36 studies, multiple sources were used. The source of the recidivism information was unknown for 14 studies. The recidivism criteria were conviction in 27 studies and arrest in 28 studies. Thirty-two studies used multiple criteria (e.g., arrest, parole violations, non-criminal justice system reports). In 2 studies, recidivism was assessed solely from self-reports, and in 6 studies the recidivism criteria was unknown. The follow-up period ranged from 12 months to 330 months, with a mean of 73 months (SD = 54.4) and a median of 60 months.
Coding procedure
Each study was coded separately by the two authors using a standard list of variables and explicit coding rules (available upon request). The categories for the predictor variables were designed to be consistent with common usage in the research literature and to limit the repetition of information from the same study. In most cases only one finding of a predictor variable was coded per sample. When multiple findings of the same variable were reported, the finding with the largest sample size was used. If the sample sizes were similar, the finding with the most complete data was selected. If the sample sizes and descriptive detail was equivalent, the median value was used.
There were several broad categories that subsumed specific variables. For example, any deviant sexual interest was a general category that was coded if the type of deviant interest was not specified. It was also coded if either sexual interest in children, sexual interest in rape, sexual interest in sadism or sexual interest in paraphilias were coded. If, for example, two or more of the specific sexual interests were coded, then the median was used when coding deviant sexual interests. When both pre-treatment and post-treatment findings were reported, the post-treatment measure was used, except in cases where the post-treatment finding was based on an insufficient number of cases. Insufficient numbers were defined as less than 30 subjects or if 50% of the subjects were lost when moving from pre to post-treatment data.
Inter-rater reliability was calculated for approximately 10% of the sample (N=10). The agreement was 86.4% (percent correct) for the sample characteristics (e.g., adults or adolescents, treated or not). Using a two-way random effects model intraclass correlation coefficients (type absolute agreement), the inter-rater reliability of the effect sizes was .83 for a single rater and .90 for the average of two raters. The actual agreement would be greater because judges conferred on their final rating. Most errors involved clerical errors or misreadings of the documents, which were simply resolved when the errors were detected. In the 10 reliability studies, Rater 1 identified 134 findings, and Rater 2 identified 131 findings, with agreement on 245 of the 265 findings identified by either rater (92.5%).
Index of predictive accuracy
The effect size indicator used was the standardized mean difference, d, defined as follows: , where M1 is the mean of the deviant group, M2 is the mean of the non-deviant group, and Sw is the pooled within standard deviation (Hasselblad & Hedges, 1995). In other words, d measures the average difference between the recidivists and the non-recidivists, and compares this difference to how much recidivists are different from other recidivists, and how much non-recidivists are different from other non-recidivists.
The d statistic was selected because it is less influenced by recidivism base rates than correlation coefficients – the other statistic commonly used in meta-analyses. Many sexual offender recidivism studies have base rates less than 10%, which restricts the magnitude of the correlations. Hanson and Bussière (1998) attempted to correct for this statistical artefact by adjusting the observed correlation based on the relative restriction of range (see also Bonta, Law & Hanson, 1998). Although this adjustment can improve the estimate of the average correlation, it can have the undesirable effect of producing large correlations based on few recidivists. Furthermore, the adjusted correlations can appear highly reliable because the variability of correlations is based on the total sample size. For example, when there is only one recidivist in a sample of 400, the variability of the correlation is considered the same as when 200 recidivists are compared with 200 non-recidivists. In contrast, the variability of d increases as the proportion of recidivists decreases, providing a more realistic estimate of the reliability of the relationship.
The formula for calculating d from various statistics were collected from diverse sources (see Appendix).
Aggregation of findings
Two methods were used to summarize the findings: median values (Slavin, 1995) and weighted mean values (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). The averaged d value, d., was calculated by weighing each di by the inverse of its variance: , where k is the number of findings, wi = 1/vi , and vi is the variance of the individual di (fixed effect model). The variance of the weighted mean was used to calculate 95% C.I.s: ; .
When di was calculated from 2 by 2 tables, the variance of di was estimated using Formula 6 from Hasselblad and Hedges (1995): . When di was calculated from other statistics (t, ROC areas, means, etc.), the variance of di was estimated using (Formula 3; Hasselblad & Hedges, 1995).
To test the generalizability of effects across studies, Hedges and Olkin's (1985) Q statistic was used: . The Q statistic is distributed as a c2 with k-1 degrees of freedom (k is the number of studies). A significant Q statistic indicates that there is more variability across studies than would be expected by chance. An individual finding (di ) was considered to be an outlier if a) it was an extreme value (highest or lowest), b) the Q statistic was significant, and c) the single finding accounted for more than 50% of the value of the Q statistic. When an outlier was detected, the results were reported with and without the exceptional case(s).
Results
The 95 studies produced 1,974 effect sizes on a combined sample of 31,216 sexual offenders (750 effects for sexual recidivism, 307 for violent non-sexual recidivism, 412 for violent recidivism and 505 for any recidivism). On average, the observed sexual recidivism rate was 13.7% (n = 20,440, 84 studies), the violent non-sexual recidivism rate was 14.0% (n = 7,444, 27 studies), the violent recidivism rate (including sexual and non-sexual violence) was 25.0% (n = 12,542, 34 studies) and the general (any) recidivism rate was 36.9% (n = 13,196, 56 studies). Studies that used artificial base rates (e.g., Dempster, 1998) were excluded from the rate calculations. The average follow-up time was 5-6 years. These figures should be considered to underestimate the real recidivism rates because not all offences are detected.
How to read the tables
Table 1 provides a broad comparison of the main categories of risk factors, followed by detailed presentations of the individual risk factors for each type of recidivism: sexual recidivism (Table 2), violent non-sexual recidivism (Table 3), any violent recidivism (sexual and non-sexual; Table 4) and general (any) recidivism (Table 5). Only predictor variables examined in at least three studies are presented. Table 6 provides a key to the studies used in the meta-analysis.
The primary consideration when estimating the importance of a risk predictor is the size of its relationship with recidivism, as indicated by the median d values and the weighted average (d.). According to Cohen (1988), d values of .20 are considered “small”, values of .50 are considered “medium”, and values of .80 are considered “large”. The value of d is approximately twice as large as the correlation coefficient calculated from the same data.
The most reliable findings are those with low variability across studies. If Q is significant, the variability is greater than would be expected by chance. With large samples sizes (greater than 1,000), even small differences between studies will be statistically significant. Another indicator of variability is the similarity between the weighted average, d., and the median. When the median and the mean suggest substantially different interpretations, then neither should be considered reliable.
When the confidence interval does not contain zero, it is equivalent to being statistically significant at p < .05. When the confidence intervals for two predictor variables do not overlap, then they can be considered statistically different from each other.
Comparison across categories of risk predictors
The broad comparison in Table 1 included all the individual variables in the detailed tables (Tables 2 through 5 are at the back) as well as relevant individual variables examined in less than three studies. When studies included multiple indicators of a larger category, the median value was selected (and the median variance). The values in Table 1 were, on average, based on 18 studies each (range 5 to 65 studies). Outliers were excluded from each category using the usual criteria (an extreme value accounting for more than 50% of the total variance).
As can be seen in the Table 1, the strongest predictors of sexual recidivism were sexual deviancy (d. = .30) and antisocial orientation (.23). The general categories of sexual attitudes (d. = .16) and intimacy deficits (d. = .15) also significantly predicted sexual recidivism, but there was substantial variation in the predictive accuracy of the subcomponents of these categories (see Table 2). The general categories of adverse childhood environment (d. = .09), general psychological problems (d. = .02) and clinical presentation (d. = -.02) had little or no relationship with sexual recidivism.
Type of Recidivism | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Category | Sexual | Violent non-sexual | Violent | Any | ||||
Sexual Deviancy |
.30 |
±.08 |
-.05 |
±.17 |
.19 |
±.08 |
.04 |
±.08 |
Antisocial Orientation |
.23 |
±.04 |
.51 |
±.07 |
.54 |
±.05 |
.52 |
±.04 |
Sexual Attitudes |
.16 |
±.12 |
.17 |
±.22 |
.14 |
±.11 |
.24 |
±.10 |
Intimacy Deficits |
.15 |
±.11 |
.12 |
±.21 |
.12 |
±.12 |
.10 |
±.10 |
Adverse Childhood Environment |
.09 |
±.08 |
-.02 |
±.17 |
.14 |
±.08 |
.11 |
±.07 |
General Psychological Problems |
.02 |
±.10 |
.21 |
±.14 |
.00 |
±.10 |
-.04 |
±.11 |
Clinical Presentation |
-.02 |
±.09 |
.16 |
±.20 |
.09 |
±.09 |
.12 |
±.08 |
Antisocial orientation (antisocial personality, antisocial traits, history of rule violation) was the major predictor of violent non-sexual recidivism (d. = .51), violent (including sexual) recidivism (d. = .54) and any recidivism (d. = .52). Although other categories were sometimes significantly related to non-sexual recidivism, the effects were much smaller that those observed for category of antisocial orientation; the next largest effect was d. = .24 for the association of sexual attitudes and any recidivism. Sexual deviancy was unrelated to violent non-sexual recidivism (d. = -.05) and general (any) recidivism (d. = .04).
Predictors of sexual recidivism
As can be seen in Table 2 (at the back), the measures of deviant sexual interests were all significantly associated with sexual recidivism: any deviant sexual interest (d. = .31), sexual interest in children (d. = .33), and paraphilic interests (d. = .21). Sexual preoccupations (paraphilic or non-paraphilic) were also significantly related to sexual recidivism (d. = .39), as were high (feminine) scores on the Masculinity-Femininity scale of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) (d. = .42).
Mixed results were found for phallometric assessment measures, which involve the direct monitoring of penile response when presented with various forms of erotic stimuli (Launay, 1999). Sexual interest in children was a significant predictor of sexual recidivism (d. = 33) as was the general category of any deviant sexual interest (d. = .24). Phallometric assessments of sexual interest in rape/violence was not significantly related to sexual recidivism, nor was the narrow category of sexual interest in boys, although the later finding was based on only 306 offenders from three studies.
Sexual recidivism was significantly predicted by almost all the indicators of antisocial orientation (antisocial personality, antisocial traits and history of rule violation). Specifically, sexual recidivism was predicted by the Hare Psychopathy Checklist (PCL-R, Hare et al., 1990, d. = .29, 13 studies), the MMPI Psychopathic deviate scale (d. = .43, 4 studies) and by other measures of antisocial personality (e.g., psychiatric diagnoses, responses to questionnaires, d. = .21, 12 studies). The general category of “any personality disorder” was also significantly related to sexual recidivism. The findings for “any personality disorder” showed more variability than would be expected by chance (Q = 45.32, p < .001), with one large study (n = 1,214; Långström, Sjöstedt & Grann, in press) finding an atypically strong relationship (d = 1.24). When this outlier was removed, the average d. was .36, and the amount of variability was no more than would be expected by chance (Q = 8.85, p > .05). Any personality disorder was grouped with measures of antisocial personality because antisocial personality was by far the most common personality disorder diagnosed among sexual offenders.
Most of the antisocial traits were related to sexual recidivism, although, as expected, the predictive accuracy of the individual traits tended to be smaller than the predictive accuracy of the general category (antisocial personality). Offenders with general self-regulation problems were more likely than offenders with stable lifestyles to sexually recidivate (d. = .37). Included among general self-regulation problems were measures of lifestyle instability, impulsivity, as well as Factor 2 from the PCL-R (Hare et al., 1990). Other antisocial traits that were significantly correlated with sexual recidivism included employment instability (d. = 22), any substance abuse (d. = .12), intoxicated during offence (d. = .11), and hostility (d. = .17).
All of the indices of rule violation were significantly related to sexual recidivism. The strongest single indicators of sexual recidivism were a) non-compliance with supervision (d. = .62), and b) violation of conditional release (d. = .50). Readers should be cautioned, however, that these effects were based on a limited number of studies and that extreme values tend to regress towards the mean (i.e., the biggest values tend to become smaller when additional data is collected).
Indicators of adverse childhood environment had very weak relationships with sexual recidivism. Notably, being sexually abused as a child was not significantly related to sexual recidivism, with a d. of .09, 95% confidence interval of -.01 to .18, based on 5,711 offenders from 17 different samples. Having been separated from biological parents was associated with increased sexual recidivism, but the effect was small (d. = .16, 95% confidence interval of .05 to .28, based on 4,145 offenders from 13 studies).
Some measures of intimacy deficits predicted sexual recidivism, whereas others did not. There was no evidence that sexual recidivism was predicted by social skills deficits (d. = -.07) or loneliness (d = .03). In contrast, sexual recidivism was predicted by emotional identification with children (having children as friends, child oriented lifestyle, d = .42) and conflicts with intimate partners (d. = .36).
The general category of “Attitudes tolerant of sexual crime” was significantly related to sexual recidivism, although the effect was small (d. = .22, 95% confidence interval of .05 to .38). The effects were not significant for child molester attitudes, low sex knowledge, or other deviant sexual attitudes (e.g., prudish attitudes toward masturbation).
None of the indicators of general psychological problems were significantly related to sexual recidivism, except in one study (Långström et al., in press). In all the other studies, sexual recidivism was unrelated to severe psychological dysfunction (psychosis) or internalizing disorders such as anxiety, and depression. On average, low self-esteem was unrelated to sexual recidivism (d. = .04) and the amount of variability was no more than would be expected by chance (Q = 10.12, p > .25).
The degree of force used in the sexual offences was significantly related to the probability of sexual recidivism, although the size of the effects was tiny (d. = .09, 95% confidence interval of .02 to .16, based on 7,221 offenders from 25 studies) and the median effect was zero. On average, the difference in recidivism rates between those who did or did not use weapons or physically injure their victims would be trivial (less than 5%). The degree of sexual intrusiveness was negatively related to sexual recidivism (d. = -.17). Those who committed non-contact sexual offences were more likely to recidivate than those who sexually touched or penetrated their victims. There was considerable variability, however, among the studies examining sexual intrusiveness, suggesting that some features of the sexual offence may be related to recidivism risk for some offenders.
None of the clinical presentation features were significantly related to sexual recidivism: lack of victim empathy (d. = -.08), denial of sex crime (d. = .02), minimization (d. = .06), and lack of motivation for treatment (assessed pre-treatment) (d. = -.08). For those who completed treatment, poor progress in treatment (d. = .14) was, on average, not significantly related to sexual recidivism. Comparisons between treatment drop-out and completers were not considered in the current review. For the above measures, the amount of variability across studies was no more than would be expected by chance.
The accuracy of sexual recidivism risk assessments
Unstructured clinical assessments predicted sexual recidivism (d. = .40, 95% confidence interval of .24 to .56), but were less accurate than actuarial risk scales specifically designed for this task (d. = .61, 95% confidence interval of .54 to .69). Although the confidence intervals overlapped, the test of the difference in predictive accuracy was statistically significant (c2 = 5.46, df = 1, p < .05). The predictive accuracy of the empirically guided approach to risk assessment ranged from d. = .41 to d. = .51, depending on whether or not to include one study with an atypically large d value of 1.30 (de Vogel, de Ruiter, van Beek & Mead, 2002). On average, the actuarial risk scales designed to predict general (any) criminal recidivism were moderate to large predictors of sexual recidivism (d. = .71, with the exception of Bonta & Hanson, 1995).
The average effect size for the unvalidated objective risk scales was large (d. = 1.06). This category included statistical risk scales developed and tested on the same sample (e.g., unreplicated multiple regression equations). Consequently, this represents the maximum predictive accuracy possible with the variables used in each study, along with a bit of luck. The predictive accuracy of these statistical risk scales would be expected to be lower when cross-validated in other samples.
The average predictive accuracy of all the individual risk scales were in the moderate to large range: VRAG (d. = .52), SORAG (d. = .48), Static-99 (d. = .63), RRASOR (d. = .59), MnSOST-R (d. = .66), and SVR-20 (d. = .77). For the other sex offender risk scales, the average d value was .66. The Statistical Information on Recidivism (SIR; Bonta, Harman, Hann & Cormier, 1996; Nuffield, 1982), developed for predicting general criminal recidivism, also predicted sexual recidivism among sexual offenders (d. = .77, or d = .52 if the atypically weak relationship in Bonta & Hanson, 1995, was included). The confidence intervals for all the risk scales overlapped, meaning that their predictive accuracies were not significantly different from each other. There was significant variability in the predictive accuracy across studies for the Static-99 (Q = 44.17, p < .01, 21 studies), RRASOR (Q = 55.84, p < .001, 18 studies) and the SVR-20 (Q = 19.01, p < .01, 6 studies). The variability of these studies could not be attributed to any single exceptional study (outlier).
Predictors of violent non-sexual recidivism
The major predictor of violent non-sexual recidivism was antisocial orientation (see Table 3). Few of the variables in the other categories (e.g., deviant sexual interests, adverse childhood environment, general psychological problems) were significantly related to violent non-sexual recidivism.
Violent non-sexual recidivism was significantly predicted by all the individual indicators of antisocial orientation (with the exception of the MMPI Pd scale). The strongest relationships were found for a history of violent crime (d. = .68), general self-regulation problems (d. = .62), and the PCL-R (d. = .57). Many of the other antisocial traits also showed effect sizes in the .40 to .50 range (e.g., employment instability, d. = .41; substance abuse, d. = .47; history of non-sexual crime, d. = .51).
None of the indicators of adverse childhood environment were significantly related to violent non-sexual recidivism. A negative relationship with father showed the strongest relationship (d. = .29), but the confidence interval was large (-.03 to .61) due to the small sample size (341, 3 studies).
Relatively few researchers have examined the relationship between intimacy deficits and violent non-sexual recidivism. Only two variables were examined in three or more studies (general people problems, negative social influences) and the confidence intervals for these variables included zero (i.e., not statistically significant). The broad category “General Psychological Functioning” was significantly related to violent non-sexual recidivism, but this result is difficult to interpret because none of the subcomponents of this category (e.g., depression, anxiety, low self-esteem) were related to violent non-sexual recidivism.
Violent non-sexual recidivism was significantly related to the degree of force used in the index sexual offence (d. = .35) and sexual intrusiveness (d. = 36). Clinical presentation variables showed weak relationships with violent non-sexual recidivism: lack of victim empathy (d. = .19, 95% confidence interval of .03 to .35), minimization (d. = .03, 95% confidence interval of -.31 to .37) and low motivation for treatment (d. = .24, 95% confidence interval of -.02 to .50). All of the above results were based on only three studies.
The accuracy of predicting violent non-sexual recidivism
The structured approaches to risk assessment showed higher predictive accuracy than the unstructured approaches. The lowest accuracy was found for clinical assessments (d. = .24), followed by empirically guided risk assessments (d. = .34), actuarial risk scales designed for sexual recidivism (d. = .44), and actuarial risk scales designed for general criminal recidivism (d. = .77). The confidence interval for the criminal recidivism risk scales (.58 to .96) did not overlap with the confidence intervals for the other approaches to risk assessment, indicating that the general criminal risk scales were more accurate in predicting violent non-sexual recidivism than the other approaches.
The individual risk scales with the strongest association with violent non-sexual recidivism were the SORAG (d. = .77) and the SIR scale (d. = .77). The Static-99 (d. = .44), RRASOR (d. = .18), and SVR-20 (d. = .35) were all significantly related to violent non-sexual recidivism, although the magnitude of their relationships were less than observed for the SORAG and SIR.
Any violent recidivism (sexual or non-sexual)
As can be seen in Table 4, measures of sexual deviance showed small, but statistically significant relationships to any violence recidivism: Any deviant sexual preferences (d. =.18), sexual preoccupations (d. = .28), and phallometrically assessed sexual interest in rape/violence (d. = .15), children (d. = .18) or any deviant sexual interest (d. = .19).
The major predictors of violent recidivism were indicators of antisocial orientation. All the indicators of antisocial personality, antisocial traits, and history of rule violation were significantly related with violent recidivism (with the exception of poor cognitive problem solving). The strongest predictors were a history of non-sexual crime (d. = .58), psychopathy (PCL-R, d. = .58) and general problems with self-regulation (d. = .52).
The indicators of adverse child environment tended to the have small relationships with violent recidivism. Any violent recidivism was significantly related to separation from parents (d. = .19) and the global category of “Childhood neglect, physical or emotional abuse” (d. = .25). Childhood sexual abuse was unrelated to violent recidivism (d. = -.05, 95% confidence interval of -.13 to .04).
With regard to intimacy deficits, there was evidence that violent recidivism was associated with “General people problems” (d. = .31) and negative social influences (d. = 29). Violent recidivism was unrelated to social skills deficits (d. = .03).
Attitudes tolerant of sex crime was not significantly related to violent recidivism, nor was low sex knowledge. None of the indicators of general psychological problems were significantly related to violent recidivism: the effect sizes ranged from -.09 for anxiety to .07 for depression. The degree of force used in the sexual offence predicted violent recidivism (d. = .22). Violent recidivism was also significantly predicted by non-contact sexual offences, although the effect was tiny (d. = .11) and there was significant variability across studies (Q = 33.08, p < .001). None of the clinical presentation variables (e.g., lack of victim empathy, minimization) significantly predicted violent recidivism.
The accuracy of predicting violent recidivism
The findings comparing different approaches to risk assessment showed considerable variability across studies. The average d value for clinical prediction was .38, but increased to .58 when the atypically low accuracy (d = .07) in Langton (2003a) was excluded. The average predictive accuracy of the sex offender risk scales was .58, with significant variability across studies (Q = 53.83, p < . 001, 15 studies). The predictive accuracy of empirically guided assessments ranged was .31, but increased to .52 when an outlier was excluded (Langton, 2003a; d = .11). The three studies that examined scales designed to predict general recidivism (e.g., SIR scale) found high levels of predictive accuracy (d. = .79, 95% confidence interval of .60 to .97) with little variability across studies (Q = 1.42, p > .50).
All the sex offender risk scales showed considerable variability across studies. For the VRAG and SORAG, the variability could be attributed to an atypically high relationship reported by Dempster (1998). Even when this outlier was excluded, the VRAG (d. = 80) and SORAG (d. =75) showed large relationships with violent recidivism, and were significantly better predictors than the Static-99 (d. = 57; c2 = 6.00, df = 1, p < .01), which, in turn was a better predictor than the RRASOR (d. = .34).
Predictors of general (any) recidivism
The pattern of results for general recidivism were very similar to the results for any violent recidivism: the most consistent predictors were measures of antisocial personality, antisocial traits and history or rule violation.
The measures of deviant sexual interest showed mixed results. Three of the four phallometric measures of deviant sexual interest significantly predicted general recidivism (any deviant sexual interest, interest in rape/violence, interest in children), but the effects were small. The general category of “Deviant sexual interest” was only significant (d. = .19) when one atypically large effect was included (Sturup, 1953). Sexual preoccupation, on the other hand, was a significant (d. = .37, 95% confidence interval of .23 to .51) and consistent (Q = 5.19, p > .25) predictor of general recidivism.
All of the indicators of antisocial orientation were significantly related with general recidivism, and most of the relationships were moderate to large. Among the strongest individual predictors were general problems with self-regulation (d. = .75), violation of conditional release (d. =.74), a history of non-violent crime (d. = .68), psychopathy (PCL-R, d. = .67), and a history of non-sexual crime (d. = .63).
Measures of adverse childhood environment showed small relationships with general recidivism, with significant relationships observed for separation from parents (d. = .27), negative relationship with mother (d. = .22), and the general category of “Childhood neglect, physical or emotional abuse” (d. = .14).
As was found for violent recidivism, the two social characteristics that were associated with general recidivism were negative social influences (d. = .24) and general people problems (d. = .17). Loneliness had no relationship with general recidivism (d. = .00, 95% confidence interval of -.11 to .11, 4 studies).
Offenders who expressed attitudes tolerant of sexual crime were at increased risk for general recidivism (d. = .29) as were those with low sexual knowledge (d. = .16). Those offenders who expressed other forms of deviant sexual attitudes (e.g., prudish attitudes towards masturbation) were at decreased risk for general recidivism (d. = -.18, 95% confidence interval of -.34 to -.02), but the finding was based on diverse measures from only three studies (n = 683). None of the measures of general psychological problems were significantly associated with general (any) recidivism: effect sizes ranged from -.08 for the general category of poor psychological functioning to .12 for severe psychological dysfunction (e.g., psychosis).
As was found for violent non-sexual recidivism, general recidivism was associated with the degree of force used in the sexual offence, and the degree of sexual intrusiveness. The clinical presentation variables showed weak associations with general recidivism. Small, significant associations were found for lack of victim empathy (d. = .12), denial (d. = .12) and poor progress in treatment (d. = .18).
Accuracy of predictions of general recidivism
The sex offender actuarial risk scales (d. = .52) were more accurate in predicting general recidivism than were unstructured clinical assessments (d. = .23) or empirically guided risk assessments (d. = .27). The most accurate method for predicting general recidivism used actuarial risk scales designed to predict general recidivism, such as the SIR scale (d. = 1.03).
The sex offender risk scale that was most strongly related to general recidivism was the SORAG (d. = .79), which was a significantly better predictor than the Static-99 (d. = .52) or SVR-20 (d. = .52). The RRASOR significantly predicted general recidivism (d. = .26), although it was a poorer predictor than the other scales (their confidence intervals did not overlap).
Discussion
The current study confirmed deviant sexual interests and antisocial orientation as important recidivism predictors for sexual offenders, and added new empirically established risk factors, including a number of factors that are potentially amenable to change (e.g., conflicts in intimate relationships, hostility). Another highlight of the review was the strong evidence for the validity of actuarial risk assessment instruments for the prediction of sexual, violent and general recidivism.
The observed recidivism rates in the current study were very similar to Hanson and Bussière's (1998) previous review. In the current study, the observed sexual recidivism rate was 13.7% after approximately 5 years (compared to 13.8% in Hanson & Bussière, 1998) and in both studies, sexual offenders were more likely to recidivate with a non-sexual offence than a sexual offence. The observed rates underestimate the actual rates because many offences remain undetected. Nevertheless, the results are consistent with other studies indicating that the overall recidivism rate of sexual offenders is lower than that observed in other samples of offenders (Langan, Schmitt & Durose, 2003; Bonta & Hanson, 1995)
Those individuals with identifiable interests in deviant sexual activities were among those most likely to continue sexual offending. The evidence was strongest for sexual interest in children and for general paraphilias (e.g., exhibitionism, voyeurism, cross-dressing). Phallometric assessments of sexual interest in rape, however, were not significantly related to sexual recidivism. The lack of relationship is somewhat surprising considering that men who have committed rape are more likely than non-rapists to respond to phallometric assessments of rape (Lalumière & Quinsey, 1994). Sexual interest in rape would have been included in the general category of “deviant sexual interests” (which was related to sexual recidivism), but our search did not find enough studies that specifically examined paraphilic rape (other than the phallometric studies) to justify separate analyses. It may be that deviant sexual motivation is more important for child molesters than for rapists, but further research is required before any strong conclusion can be made about the validity of paraphilic rape as a predictor of sexual recidivism.
One interesting risk predictor was sexual preoccupations (high rates of sexual interests and activities), which significantly predicted sexual, violent, and general recidivism. Kafka (1997) found high rates of masturbation, pornography use, and impersonal sex among sexual offenders referred to his clinic. There are several possible connections between sexual preoccupations and sexual offending, including a general lack of self-control (common among young people and general criminals), specific problems controlling sexual impulses, and a tendency to overvalue sex in the pursuit of happiness. Kanin (1967), for example, found that adolescent males who believed that sexual activities were important for gaining peer status were at increased risk for committing rape. Kafka (2003) has proposed that the sexual activation of sexual offenders may be linked to monoaminergic neuro-regulatory mechanisms, but a biological basis for the sexual preoccupations of sexual offenders has yet to be established (Haake et al., 2003).
As in previous reviews, all forms of recidivism were predicted by an unstable, antisocial lifestyle, characterized by rule violations, poor employment history and reckless, impulsive behaviour (Bonta et al., 1998; Gendreau, Little & Goggin, 1996; Quinsey et al., 1995). Antisocial orientation was a particularly important predictor of violent and general recidivism, but indicators of antisocial orientation were also among the largest predictors of sexual recidivism (e.g., non-compliance with supervision, violation of conditional release). Lack of self-control may directly lead to a wide range of criminal behaviour (e.g., Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990), and it could also be specifically linked to recidivism because high levels of self-regulation are required to change dysfunctional habits.
The review identified some new predictor variables that may be of interest to those attempting to change sexual offenders' recidivism risk. Previous research has established that lack of an intimate partner was associated with increased risk for sexual recidivism (Hanson & Bussière, 1998). In the current review, the importance of intimate relationships was further reinforced by the finding that sexual recidivism was also associated with conflicts in intimate relationships. Another intimacy variable associated with sexual recidivism was emotional identification with children. This pattern, most commonly found among extrafamilial child molesters, describes individuals who feel emotionally closer to children than adults, and who have children as friends. They may also have child oriented lifestyles, and feel like children themselves (Wilson, 1999).
The association between intimacy deficits and persistent sexual offending is consistent with several current explanations for sexual offending (Malamuth, 2003; Ward, Hudson, Marshall & Siegert, 1995). How intimacy deficits influence offending, however, remains an important research question. One hypothesis is that child molesters turn to child sexual partners because they lack the skills to relate to adults and, consequently, feel lonely. Although low social skills and loneliness are common among sex offenders (Seidman, Marshall, Hudson & Robertson, 1994), neither of these factors were associated with increased risk for sexual recidivism in the current review. It is possible that social inadequacy and rejection are less important to offending than are the strategies used to address such problems (e.g., turning to children).
A similar explanation could apply to the observations that general psychological problems (e.g., depression, anxiety) are common among sexual offenders (Raymond, Coleman, Ohlerking, Christensen & Miner, 1999), but that general psychological problems have little or no relationship to recidivism. Many suffer with psychological distress, but sexual offenders are more likely than other groups to respond to stress through sexual acts and fantasies (deviant or otherwise) (Cortoni & Marshall, 2001). Although chronic negative mood is unrelated to recidivism, acute deterioration of mood is associated with increases in deviant fantasies among sexual offenders (McKibben, Proulx, & Lusignan, 1994) and may signal the time period when offending is most likely (Hanson & Harris, 2000b). Again, it is not the distress itself, but the attempted solution that is the problem.
It is also possible that the factors associated with becoming a sexual offender are different from the factors associated with persistence. The personal histories of sexual offenders are frequently marked by physical, sexual and emotional abuse (Lee, Jackson, Pattison & Ward, 2002; Smallbone & Dadds, 1998). Indices of an aversive childhood environment, however, had minimal relationships with sexual or non-sexual recidivism in the current review. The most promising developmental predictor was separation from parents, but this factor can partly be considered a consequence of early behaviour problems (e.g., placement in juvenile detention).
Attitudes tolerant of sexual assault showed a small, but statistically significant, relationship with sexual recidivism. Although consistent with theory, the utility of attitudes as a risk predictor in applied settings needs to be considered with caution. There was significant variability across studies and the four studies that specifically examined attitudes tolerant of adult-child sex did not find a significant relationship to sexual recidivism (95% confidence interval of -.16 to .33). The typical attitude measures (self-report or interviews) are highly influenced by the context of the assessment. It may be that attitudes expressed within relationships of trust (e.g., in treatment) are more reliable risk indicators than those expressed in adversarial contexts (e.g., civil commitment hearings). It will be interesting to know whether significant associations with recidivism will be found for attitudes assessed using difficult-to-fake measures, such as the implicit attitude test (Gray, MacCulloch, Smith & Snowdon, 2002; Greenwald, McGhee & Schwartz, 1998) or the Stroop task (Smith & Waterman, in press).
The clinical presentation variables (e.g., denial, low victim empathy, low motivation for treatment) had little or no relationship with sexual or non-sexual recidivism. As with pro-offending attitudes, it may be difficult to assess sincere remorse in criminal justice settings. It is also possible that evaluators looking for risk factors have little to gain from listening to offenders' attempt to justify their transgressions. Psychotherapists often consider full disclosure desirable, and courts are lenient towards those who show remorse; few of us, however, are inclined to completely reveal our faults and transgressions. Research has also suggested that full disclosure of negative personal characteristics is associated with negative social outcomes, including poor progress in psychotherapy (Kelly, 2000). Consequently, resistance to being labelled a sexual offender may not be associated with increased recidivism risk, even though it does create barriers to engagement in treatment. Offenders who minimize their crimes are at least indicating that sexual offending is wrong.
None of the individual risk factors were sufficiently prognostic to be used in isolation; consequently, prudent evaluators need to consider a variety of factors. There were three common approaches to combining individual risk factors into an overall assessment: a) unguided professional judgement (based on expertise and unique features of the case), b) empirically-guided professional judgements (structured around empirically established risk factors), and c) pure actuarial prediction (the risk factors and the method of combining the factors being determined in advance).
Risk assessments were most likely to be accurate when they were constrained by empirical evidence. Unstructured clinical assessments were significantly related to recidivism, but their accuracy was consistently less than that of actuarial measures. The predictive accuracy of clinical assessments was slightly higher in the current review (d. = .40; k = 9) than in Hanson and Bussière's earlier review (r = .10; d ≈ .20, k =10), although the difference was not statistically significant (their confidence intervals overlap). Part of the improvement involved inclusion of a recent study in which clinical assessment did quite well (Hood, Shute, Feilzer & Wilcox, 2002), and removing to a separate category post-treatment assessments of “benefit from treatment”. The extent to which recent advances in research knowledge have improved routine clinical assessments remains unknown.
Empirically-guided professional judgements showed predictive accuracies that were intermediate between the values observed for clinical assessments and pure actuarial approaches. The same pattern of results applied to the prediction of sexual recidivism, violent non-sexual recidivism, and general (any) recidivism. There were no sex offender recidivism studies that examined the accuracy of risk assessments in which judges were presented with actuarial results and then allowed to adjust their overall predictions based on external risk factors (Webster et al., 1994). Future research should consider such adjusted actuarial risk assessments because this approach has proven the most accurate in other domains (e.g., weather forecasting; Swets, Dawes & Monahan, 2000).
For sexual recidivism, the predictive accuracies of the actuarial risk scales were in the moderate to large range. There were no significant differences among the sex offender specific measures (e.g., SORAG, Static-99, RRASOR). Although based on few studies, it appeared that the measures designed to predict general criminal recidivism (e.g., SIR, Bonta et al., 1996) predicted sexual recidivism as well as did the measures specifically designed for sexual or violent recidivism (e.g., SORAG, Static-99). Some relationship between general criminality and sexual recidivism would be expected, given that antisocial orientation is a well-established risk marker.
For the prediction of general recidivism, the general criminal risk scales were superior (d. = 1.03) to measures designed to predict sexual recidivism (d. = .52). The general criminal risk scales were also as good at predicting any violent recidivism (d. = 79) as the scales specifically designed to assess violent recidivism among sexual offenders (SORAG, d. = .75/.81). Further research is required to determine whether the specific sexual offender risk scales provide useful information that is not already captured in the general criminal risk scales.
Since Hanson and Bussière's (1998) previous review, researchers have increasingly focussed on risk factors that are, in theory, changeable. The current review found that a number of these potentially dynamic risk factors were significantly related to sexual recidivism (e.g., sexual preoccupations, conflicts in intimate relationships, hostility, emotional identification with children, attitudes tolerant of sexual assault). What remains unknown is whether changes on such factors are associated with reductions in recidivism risk. In general, evaluations of treatment progress showed little relationship to recidivism, with an average d. of .14. Nevertheless, there were some recent examples in which ratings of progress in treatment were significantly related to recidivism (Beech, Erikson, Friendship & Ditchfield, 2001, d = .50; Marques, Day, Wiederanders & Nelson, 2002, d = .55). Both of these studies used highly structured approaches to evaluating treatment gains, and were informed by empirical research concerning relevant risk factors.
Readers familiar with Hanson and Bussière's (1998) earlier review will observe many minor differences between the earlier results and the current results. For example, deviant sexual interest in children as measured by phallometry was the largest single predictor of sexual recidivism in the earlier review (average r = .32, d ≈ .60, k = 7), whereas the effect was smaller in the current review (d. = .32, k = 10). Similarly, employment instability significantly predicted sexual recidivism in the current review (d. = .22, k = 15) but the effect was not significant in the previous review (average r = .07, d. ≈ .15, k = 5). Some of the changes would be due to the inclusion of additional research studies and the use of different (hopefully better) statistics. As research accumulates, it is possible to further examine the boundary conditions of specific risk factors (e.g., variations across samples, assessment methods). It is worth remembering, however, that even with large sample sizes, random variation is expected. Three studies was the minimum for inclusion in this meta-analysis, but it is far from the minimum required to obtain immutable results.
Meta-analyses provide broad overviews, and can easily neglect potentially important differences between studies. The definitions of constructs varied across studies, as did the samples. The current study focussed on mixed groups of sexual offenders, and there was no effort to identify distinct predictors for specific subgroups (e.g., rapists, exhibitionists). Nevertheless, the findings were remarkably consistent. For 70% of the findings, the amount of variability across studies was no more than would be expected by chance (p < .05). Furthermore, there was substantial consistency within many of the categories of predictors, with almost all of the variables being significant (e.g., sexual deviancy, history or rule violation) or non-significant (e.g., general psychological problems, clinical presentation). There is still substantial variability across studies that remains to be explained, but it appears that research is getting closer to identifying the constructs that are, and are not, related to recidivism among sexual offenders.
Author Note
We would like to thank the following researchers who provided raw data or findings not included in other reports: S. Brown, H. Gretton, G. Harris, N. Långström, C. Langton, R. Lieb, J. Marques, M. Miner, L. Motiuk, T. Nicholaichuk, J. Proulx, J. Reddon, M. Rice, G. Schiller, L. Studer, L. Song, and D. Thornton. The help of R. Broadhurst, R. Dempster, R. Kropp, L. Hartwell, A. Hills, N. Morvan and L. Rasmussen in locating unpublished documents was greatly appreciated. Thanks also to Dale Arnold, Jim Bonta, Guy Bourgon, Don Grubin, Amy Phenix, Vern Quinsey, Marnie Rice and David Thornton who provided comments on earlier versions of the manuscript.
The views expressed are those of the authors and are not necessarily those of Public Safety and Emergency Preparedness Canada. Correspondence should be addressed to R. Karl Hanson, Corrections Research, Public Safety and Emergency Preparedness Canada, 340 Laurier Ave., West, Ottawa, K1A 0P8. Email: hansonk@sgc.gc.ca.
References
References marked with an asterisk were included in the meta-analysis
*Abel, G. G., Mittelman, M., Becker, J. V., Rathner, J., & Rouleau, J. L. (1988). Predicting child molesters' response to treatment. In R.A. Prentky & V. L. Quinsey (Eds.), Human sexual aggression: Current prespectives (pp. 223-234). New York: New York Academy of Science.
* Allam, J. (1999). Effective practice in work with sex offenders: A re-conviction study comparing treated and untreated offenders. Birmingham, West Midlands: West Midlands Probation Service Sex Offender Unit.
Andrews, D. A., & Bonta, J. (2003). The psychology of criminal conduct (3rd ed.). Cincinnati: Anderson.
* Bakker, L., Hudson, S., Wales, D., & Riley, D. (2002). And then there was light: Evaluating the Kia Marama treament programme for New Zealand sex offenders against children. Christchurch, NZ: Department of Corrections.
* Barbaree, H. E., & Marshall, W. L. (1988). Deviant sexual arousal, offense history, and demographic variables as predictors of reoffense among child molesters. Behavioural Sciences & the Law, 6, 267-280.
* Barbaree, H. E., & Seto, M. C. (1998). The ongoing follow-up of sex offenders treated at the Warkworth Sexual Behaviour Clinic (Research Report). Toronto, Canada: Forensic Program, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health.
* Barbaree, H. E., Seto, M. C., Langton, C. M., & Peacock, E. J. (2001). Evaluating the predictive accuracy of six risk assessment instruments for adult sex offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 28, 490-521.
* Barbaree, H. E., Seto, M. C., & Maric, A. (1996). Working papers in impulsivity research: Sex offender characteristics, response to treatment, and correctional release decisions at the Warkworth Sexual Behaviour Clinic (Research Report). Toronto, Canada: Forensic Division, Clarke Institute of Psychiatry.
* Bartosh, D. L., Garby, T., & Lewis, D. (2003). Differences in the predictive validity of actuarial risk assessments in relation to sex offender type. International Journal of Offender Therapy & Comparative Criminology, 47(4), 422-438.
* Beech, A., Erikson, M., Friendship, C., & Ditchfield, J. (2001). A six-year follow-up of men going through probation-based sex offender treatment programmes (Findings #144). London, U.K.: Home Office.
Beech, A. R., Fisher, D. D., & Thornton, D. (2003). Risk assessment of sex offenders. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 34, 339-352.
*Beech, A., Friendship, C., Erikson, M., & Hanson, R. K. (2002). The relationship between static and dynamic risk factors and reconviction in a sample of U.K. child abusers. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 14 (2), 155-167.
Beech, A., & Ward, T. (in press). The integration of etiology and risk in sexual offenders: A theoretical framework. Aggression and Violent Behavior.
* Belanger, N., & Earls, C. (1996). Sex offender recidivism prediction. Forum on Corrections Research, 8(2), 22-24.
* Bench, L. L., Kramer, S. P., & Erickson, S. (1997). A discriminant analysis of predictive factors in sex offender recidivism. In B. K. Schwartz & H. R. Cellini (Eds.), The sex offender: New insights, treatment innovations and legal developments (pp. 15-1 - 15-15). Kingston, NJ: Civic Research Institute.
*Berger, P. (2002, September). The role of personality disorders and paraphilias as predictors of relapse in male sexual offenders. Presentation at the 7th conference of the International Association for the Treatment of Sexual Offenders, Vienna, Austria.
Boer, D. P., Hart, S. D., Kropp, P. R., & Webster, C. D. (1997). Manual for the Sexual Violence Risk – 20. Vancouver, BC: The British Columbia Institute Against Family Violence.
*Bonta, J. & Hanson, R.K. (1995). [Ten year recidivism data for offenders released from the Correctional Service of Canada in 1983/1984]. Unpublished raw data.
Bonta, J., Harman, W.G., Hann, R.G., & Cormier, R.B. (1996). The prediction of recidivism among federally sentenced offenders: A re-validation of the SIR scale. Canadian Journal of Criminology, 38, 61-79.
Bonta, J., Law, M., & Hanson, R. K. (1998). The prediction of criminal and violent recidivism among mentally disordered offenders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 123-142.
*Bradford, J., Firestone, P., Fernandez, M., Curry, S., & Larose, M. (1997). Predictors of recidivism in a population of Canadian sex offenders: Psychological, physiological, and offense factors. Unpublished manuscript.
*Broadhurst, R. (1999). Probabilities of sex offender recidivism: A survival analysis. Unpublished manuscript.
*Broadhurst, R., & Loh, N. (1997, March). Careers of sex offenders: The probabilities of re-arrest. Paper presented at the Australian Institute of Criminology conference, Canberra, Australia.
*Broadhurst, R.G. & Maller, R.A. (1992). The recidivism of sex offenders in the Western Australian prison population. British Journal of Criminology, 32, 54-80.
*Buffington-Vollum, J., Edens, J. F., Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, J. K. (2002). Psychopathy as a predictor of institutional misbehaviour among sex offenders: A prospective replication. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 29(5), 497-511.
California Office of the Attorney General. (2003). Sex offender statistics as of December 2, 2003. Retrieved January 12, 2004, from http://caag.state.ca.us/megan/pdf/0312chart.pdf.
Campbell, T. W. (2000). Sexual predator evaluations and phrenology: Considering issues of evidentiary reliability. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 18, 111-130.
Caspi, A., Moffit, T. E., Silva, P. A., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Krueger, R. F., & Schmutte, P. S. (1994). Are some people crime-prone? Replications of the personality-crime relationship across countries, genders, races, and methods. Criminology, 32, 163-195.
*Christiansen, K. O., Elers-Nielsen, M., Le Maire, L., & Sturup, G. K. (1965). Recidivism among sexual offenders. Scandinavian Studies in Criminology, 1, 55-85.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
*Cooper, H. (2000). Long-term follow-up of a community-based treatment program for adolescent sex offenders. Unpublished master's thesis, Lakehead University, Thunder Bay, Ontario, Canada.
Cortoni, F., & Marshall, W. L. (2001). Sex as a coping strategy and its relationship to juvenile sexual history and intimacy in sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 13, 27-43.
*Davis, G. L., Hoffman, R. G. & Stacken, N. (1991, August). 2-4 Year Post-Incarceration Follow-up of Treated Sex Offenders. Paper presentation at the 99th meeting of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, CA.
*Dempster, R. J. (1998). Prediction of sexually violent recidivism: A comparison of risk assessment instruments. Unpublished master's thesis, Simon Fraser University, British Columbia, Canada.
*Dempster, R. J., & Hart, S. D. (2002). The relative utility of fixed and variable risk factors in discriminating sexual recidivists and nonrecidivists. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 14(2), 121-138.
*de Vogel, V., de Ruiter, C., van Beek, D., & Mead, G. (2002). Predictive validity of structured and actuarial risk assessment for sexual violence: A retrospective study in a Dutch sample of treated sex offenders. Submitted for publication.
*Dix, G. E. (1976). Differential processing of abnormal sex offenders: Utilization of California's Mentally Disordered Sex Offenders: Utilization of California's mentally disordered sex offender program. Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 67, 233-243.
*Doshay, L. J. (1943). The boy sex offender and his later career. Montclair, NJ: Patterson Smith.
*Ducro, C., Claix, A., & Pham, Th. H. (2002, September). Assessment of the Static-99 in a Belgian sex offender forensic population. Poster presented at the European Conference on Psychology and Law, Leuven, Belgium.
*Dwyer, S. M. (1997). Treatment outcome study: Seventeen years after sexual offender treatment. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 9, 149-160.
*Epperson, D. L., Kaul, J. D., & Hesselton, D. (1998). Minnesota Sex Offender Screening Tool – Revised (MnSOST-R): Development, performance, and recommended risk level cut scores. Iowa State University & Minnesota Department of Corrections.
*Epperson, D. L., Kaul, J. D., & Huot, S. J. (1995, October). Predicting risk of recidivism for incarcerated sex offenders: Updated development on the Sex Offender Screening Tool (SOST). Paper presented at the 14th annual Research and Treatment Conference of the Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, New Orleans, Louisiana.
*Epperson, D. L., Kaul, J. D., Huot, S. J., Hesselton, D., Alexander, W. & Goldman, R. (2000, November). Cross-validation of the Minnesota Sex Offender Screening Tool-Revised. Paper presented at the 19th annual conference of the Association for theTreatment of Sexual Abusers, San Diego, California. Retrieved October 16, 2001, from http://psych-server.iastate.edu/faculty/epperson/atsa2000/index.htm
*Federoff, J. P., Wisner-Carlson, R., Dean, S., & Berlin, F. S. (1992). Medroxy-progesterone acetate in the treatment of paraphilic sexual disorders. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 18, 109-123.
Firestone, P., Bradford, J. M., Greenberg, D. M., & Serran, G. A. (2000). The relationship between deviant sexual arousal and psychopathy in incest offenders, extrafamilial child molesters, and rapists. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 28, 303-308.
*Firestone, P., Bradford, J. M., McCoy, M., Greenberg, D. M., Curry, S., & Larose, M. R. (1998). Recidivism in convicted rapists. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry & Law, 26(2), 185-200.
*Firestone, P., Bradford, J. M., McCoy, M., Greenberg, D. M., Curry, S., & Larose, M. R. (2000). Prediction of recidivism in extrafamilial child molesters based on court-related assessments. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 12, 203-221.
*Firestone, P., Bradford, J. M., McCoy, M., Greenberg, D. M., Larose, M. R., & Curry, S. (1999). Prediction of recidivism in incest offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 511-531.
*Fischer, D. R. (2000). Sex offender risk assessment validation study. Phoenix, AZ: Arizona Department of Corrections.
*Fitch, J. H. (1962). Men convicted of sexual offences against children: A descriptive follow-up study. British Journal of Criminology, 3, 18-37.
Fleiss, J. L. (1994). Measures of effect size for categorical data. In H. Cooper & L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis (pp. 245-260). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
*Florida Department of Health and Rehabilitative Services. (1985). Status of the sex offender treatment programs, fiscal year 1983-84 (Annual Report to the Florida Legislature). Tampa, FL: Author.
*Frisbie, L. V., & Dondis, E. H. (1965). Recidivism among treated sex offenders (California Mental Health Monograph No. 5). California: State of California Department of Mental Hygiene.
Gendreau, P., Little, T., & Goggin, C. (1996). A meta-analysis of the predictors of adult offender recidivism: What works! Criminology, 34, 575-607.
*Gfellner, B. M. (2000). Adult sex offenders in treatment programs: Predictors of success and recidivism (Research report to the Sex Offender Treatment Advisory Group (SOTAG)). Brandon, Manitoba.
*Gibbens, T. C. N., Soothill, K. L. & Way, C. K. (1978). Sibling and parent child incest offenders: A long term follow-up. British Journal of Criminology, 18, 40-52.
*Gore, D. K. (1988). Measuring the cognitive distortions of child molesters: Psychometric properties of the Cognition Scale. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Georgia State University, Georgia.
Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Gray, N. S., MacCulloch, M., Smith, J., & Snowdon, R. J. (2002, September). Abnormal associations between children and sex in paedophiles as measured by the covert association task (CAT). Presentation at the 7th Biennial Conference of the International Association for the Treatment of Sexual Offenders, Vienna.
*Greenberg, D. M., Da Silva, J., & Loh, N. (2002). Evaluation of the Western Australian sex offender treatment unit (1987-1999): A quantitative analysis. Western Australia: University of Western Australia, Forensic Research Unit, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Sciences.
*Greenberg, D. M., Firestone, P., Bradford, J. M., & Broom, I. (2000). Infantophiles. In L. B. Schlesinger (Ed.), Serial offenders: Current thought, recent findings (pp. 229-246). NY: CRC Press. 62.6
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, J. L., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in social cognition: The implicit attitudes test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480.
*Gretton, H. M. (1995). [The relationship between phallometric assessment and recidivism among 185 juvenile sex offenders]. Unpublished raw data.
*Gretton, H. M., McBride, M., Hare, R. D., O'Shaughnessy, R., & Kumka, G. (2001). Psychopathy and recidivism in adolescent sex offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 28, 427-499.
Haake, P., Schedlowski, M., Exton, M. S., Giepen, C., Hartmann, U., Osterheider, M., Flesch, M., Janssen, O. E., Leygraf, N., & Krueger, T. H. C. (2003). Acute neuroendocrine response to sexual stimulation in sexual offenders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 48, 265-271.
*Hall, G. C. N. (1988). Criminal behavior as a function of clinical and actuarial variables in a sexual offender population. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 773-775.
*Hanlon, M. J., Larson, S., & Zacher, S. (1999). The Minnesota SOST and sexual reoffending in North Dakota: A retrospective study. International Journal of Offender Therapy & Comparative Criminology, 43(1), 71-77.
Hanson, R. K. (1997). The development of a brief actuarial risk scale for sexual offense recidivism. (User Report 97-04). Ottawa: Department of the Solicitor General of Canada.
*Hanson, R. K. (2002). [Evaluation of Manitoba's Secondary Risk Assessment]. Unpublished raw data.
Hanson, R. K., & Bussière, M. T. (1996). Predictors of sexual offender recidivism: A meta-analysis. (User Report 96-04). Ottawa: Department of the Solicitor General of Canada.
Hanson, R. K., & Bussière, M. T. (1998). Predicting relapse: A meta-analysis of sexual offender recidivism studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(2), 348-362.
Hanson, R. K., Gordon, A., Harris, A. J. R., Marques, J. K., Murphy, W., Quinsey, V. L., & Seto, M. C. (2002). First report of the Collaborative Outcome Data Project on the effectiveness of psychological treatment for sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 14(2), 169-194.
*Hanson, R. K., & Harris, A. J. R. (2000a). [Dynamic predictors of sexual recidivism based on comparing 208 recidivists and 201 non-recidivists on community supervision]. Unpublished raw data.
Hanson, R. K., & Harris, A. J. R. (2000b). Where should we intervene? Dynamic predictors of sex offense recidivism. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27, 6-35.
Hanson, R. K., & Harris, A. J. R. (2001). A structured approach to evaluating change among sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 13(2), 105-122.
*Hanson, R. K., Steffy, R. A. & Gauthier, R. (1992). Long-term follow-up of child molesters: Risk predictors and treatment outcome (Research Report No. 1992-02). Ottawa, Canada: Solicitor General of Canada.
*Hanson, R. K., Steffy, R. A., Scott, H., & Gauthier, R. (1993). [Long-term recidivism of child molesters released from Millbrook Institution between 1958 & 1974]. Unpublished raw data.
Hanson, R. K., & Thornton, D. (2000). Improving risk assessments for sex offenders: A comparison of three actuarial scales. Law and Human Behavior, 24(1), 119-136.
Hare, R.D., Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., Forth, A. E., Hart, S. D. & Newman, J. P. (1990). The Revised Psychopathy Checklist: Reliability and factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 2, 338-341.
*Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., Quinsey, V. L., Lalumiere, M. L., Boer, D., & Lang, C. (2003). A multi-site comparison of actuarial risk instruments for sex offenders. Psychological Assessment, 15(3), 413-425.
*Hartwell, L. L. (2001). Sex Offender Risk Appraisal Guide: Validity and Utility for Hawai'i Sex Offender Risk Assessments. Unpublished clinical research project, American School of Professional Psychology, Hawai'i Campus.
Hasselblad, V. & Hedges, L. V. (1995). Meta-analysis of screening and diagnostic tests. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 167-178.
*Haynes, A. K., Yates, P. M., Nicholaichuk, T., Gu, D., & Bolton, R. (2000, June). Sexual deviancy, risk and recidivism: The relationship between deviant arousal, the RRASOR and sexual recidivism. Paper presented at the annual conference of the Canadian Psychological Association, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada.
*Hecker, J., Scoular, J., Righthand, S., & Nangle, D. (2002, October). Predictive Validity of the J-SOAP over 10-plus years: Implications for risk assessment. Poster presented at the 21st annual conference of the Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. New York: Academic Press.
*Hildebrand, M., de Ruiter, C., & de Vogel, V. (in press). Psychopathy and sexual deviance in treated rapists: Association with (sexual) recidivism. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment.
*Hills, A. L. (2002). Assessment, treatment, and recidivism of Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal offenders: A comparison of intra-familial and extra-familial male sexual offenders in Saskatchewan. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Regina, Regina, Saskatchewan, Canada.
*Hood, R., Shute, S., Feilzer, M., & Wilcox, A. (2002). Sex offenders emerging from long-term imprisonment: A study of their long-term reconviction rates and of parole board members' judgments of their risk. British Journal of Criminology, 42(2), 371-394.
*Hudson, S. M., Wales, D. S., Bakker, L., & Ward, T. (2002). Dynamic risk factors: The Kia Marama evaluation. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 14(2), 103-119.
Kafka, M. P. (1997). Hypersexual desire in males: An operational definition and clinical implications for men with paraphilias and paraphilia-related disorders. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 26, 505-526.
Kafka, M. P. (2003). The monoamine hypothesis for the pathophysiology of paraphilic disorders: An update. In R. A. Prentky, E. S. Janus & M. C. Seto (Eds.), Sexually coercive behavior: Understanding and management (pp. 86-94). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 989. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
*Kahn, T. J., & Chambers, H. J. (1991). Assessing reoffense risk with juvenile sexual offenders. Child Welfare, 70, 333-345.
Kanin, E. J. (1967). Reference groups and sex conduct norm violations. The Sociological Quarterly, 8, 495-504.
Kelly, A. E. (2000). Helping construct desirable identities: A self-presentational view of psychotherapy. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 475-494.
*Khanna, A., Brown, P., Malcolm, P. B., & Williams, S. M. (1989, March). Outcome data on sex offenders assessed and treated at Regional Treatment Centre (Ontario). Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Special Needs Offenders Conference, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada.
Knight, R. A., & Sims-Knight, J. E. (2003). The developmental antecedents of sexual coercion against women: Testing alternative hypotheses with structural equation modeling. In R. A. Prentky, E. S. Janus & M. Seto (Eds.), Sexually coercive behavior: Understanding and management (pp. 72-85). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciencies, Vol. 989.
*Kozol, H. L., Boucher, R. J., & Garofalo, R. F. (1972). The diagnosis and treatment of dangerousness. Crime and Delinquency, 18, 371-392.
*Kropp, P. R. (2000, November). The Risk for Sexual Violence Protocol (RSVP). Paper presented at the annual conference of the Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, San Diego, CA.
*Lab, S. P., Shields, G., & Schondel, C. (1993). Research note: An evaluation of juvenile sexual offender treatment. Crime and Delinquency, 39, 543-553.
Lalumière, M. & Quinsey, V. L. (1994). The discriminability of rapists from non-sex offenders using phallometric measures : A meta-analysis. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 21, 150-175.
Langan, P. A., Schmitt, E. L., & Durose, M. R. (2003). Recidivism of sex offenders released from prison in 1984. Bureau of Justice Statistics NCJ 198281. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
*Langevin, R. & Fedoroff, J. P. (2000). 25 year recidivism study of sex offenders. Unpublished manuscript.
*Långström, N. (2002a). Long-term follow-up of criminal recidivism in young offenders: Temporal patterns and risk factors. Psychology, Crime & the Law, 8, 41-58.
* Långström, N. (2002b). [Relationship of denial to recidivism among 46 juvenile sex offenders in Sweden]. Unpublished raw data.
* Långström, N. (in press). Accuracy of actuarial procedures for assessment of sexual offender recidivism risk may vary across ethnicity. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment.
*Långström, N., & Grann, M. (2000). Risk for criminal recidivism among young sex offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15, 855-871.
*Långström, N., Sjöstedt, G., & Grann, M. (in press). Psychiatric disorders and recidivism among sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment.
*Langton, C. M. (2003a). Contrasting approaches to risk assessment with adult male sexual offenders: An evaluation of recidivism prediction schemes and the utility of supplementary clinical information for enhancing predictive accuracy. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Institute of Medical Science, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
*Langton, C. M. (2003b). [The relationship between progress in treatment and recidivism among 444 sexual offenders released from Warkworth Institution]. Unpublished raw data.
Launay, G. (1999). The phallometric measurement of offenders: An update. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 9, 254-274.
Lee, J., Jackson, H., Pattison, P., & Ward, T. (2002). Developmental risk factors for sexual offending. Child Abuse & Neglect, 26, 73-92.
Lund, C. A. (2000). Predictors of sexual recidivism: Did meta-analysis clarify the role and relevance of denial? Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 12, 275-287.
*Mair, K. J. & Stevens, R. H. (1994). Offending histories and offending behaviour: A ten year follow up of sex offenders tried by sheriff and district courts in Grampian, Scotland. Psychology, Crime & Law, 1, 83-92.
*Mair, K. J. & Wilson, D. (1994). Prevalence, prediction and perseveration of sexual reoffending in a sample identified through court records. Unpublished manuscript.
*Mair, K. J., & Wilson, D. (1995). Sexual reconviction over time in Scottish sex offenders identified through court records. Unpublished manuscript.
Malamuth, N. M. (2003). Criminal and noncriminal sexual aggressors : Integrating psychopathy in heirarchical-mediational confluence model. In R. A. Prentky, E. S. Janus & M. Seto (Eds.), Sexually coercive behavior: Understanding and management (pp. 33-58). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 989.
*Malcolm, P. B., Andrews, D. A., & Quinsey, V. L. (1993). Discriminant and predictive validity of phallometrically measured sexual age and gender preference. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 8, 486-501.
Maletzky, B. M. (1993). Factors associated with success and failure in the behavioral and cognitive treatment of sexual offenders. Annals of Sex Research, 6, 241-258.
*Marques, J. K. & Day, D. M. (1996). [SOTEP follow-up data for 1995]. Unpublished raw data.
*Marques, J. K., Day, D. M., Wiederanders, M., & Nelson, C. (2002, October). Main effects and beyond: New findings from California's Sex Offender Treatment & Evaluation Project (SOTEP). Paper presented at the 21st annual conference of the Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
Marshall, P. (1997). The prevalence of convictions for sexual offending. Research Finding No. 55. Research and Statistics Directorate. Home Office: London.
*Marshall, W. L., & Barbaree, H. E. (1988). The long-term evaluation of a behavioral treatment program for child molesters. Behaviour, Research and Therapy, 26, 499-511.
*McBride, M., Gretton, H., Hare, R. D. (1995, October). Familial risk factors, psychopathy and recidivism in adolescent sexual offenders. . Paper presented at the annual conference of the Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, New Orleans, LA.
*McGrath, R. J., Cumming, G., Livingston, J. A., & Hoke, S. E. (2003). Outcome of a treatment program for adult sex offenders: From prison to community. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18 (1), 3-17.
McKibben, A., Proulx, J., & Lusignan, R. (1994). Relationships between conflict, affect and deviant sexual behaviours in rapists and pedophiles. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 13, 571-575.
*Meyer, L. C., & Romero, J. (1980). A ten-year follow-up of sex offender recidivism. Unpublished manuscript.
*Meyer, W. J., Cole, C., & Emory, E. (1992). Depo Provera treatment for sex offending behaviour: An evaluation of outcome. Bulletin of the American Academy of Psychiatry and Law, 20, 249-259.
*Miner, M. H. (2002a). Factors associated with recidivism in juveniles: An analysis of serious juvenile sex offenders. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 39 (4), 421-436.
*Miner, M. H. (2002b). [Factors associated with recidivism among 129 juvenile sex offenders released from Minnesota Corrections]. Unpublished raw data.
*Minnesota Department of Corrections. (1999). Community-based sex offender program evaluation project: 1999 report to the legislature. St. Paul, MN: Author.
*Mohr. J. W., Turner, R. E., & Jerry, M. B. (1964). Pedophilia and exhibitionism. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
*Money, J. & Bennett, R. G. (1981). Post adolescent paraphilic sex offenders: Antiandrogenic and counseling therapy follow-up. International Journal of Mental Health, 10, 122-133.
*Morton, K. E. (2003). [Recidivism prediction in a sample of male adolescent sexual offenders assessed at a community sexual offender treatment program]. Unpublished raw data.
*Motiuk, L. L. & Brown, S. L. (1995). [Survival time for sexual offenders released from the Correctional Services of Canada in 1991 & 1994]. Unpublished raw data.
*Mulloy, R., & Smiley, W. C. (1996). Recidivism and treated sex offenders. Presentation at the International Congress of Psychologists, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
*Mulloy, R., & Smiley, W. C. (1997, June). The specific impact of REBT-Based treatment on sex offenders. Poster presented at the annual conference of the Canadian Psychological Association, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
*Nicholaichuk, T. (1997). [Relationship between a modified version of the RRASOR and sexual recidivism among 283 untreated sexual offenders]. Unpublished raw data.
*Nicholaichuk, T. (2001, November). The comparison of two standardized risk assessment instruments in a sample of Canadian Aboriginal sexual offenders. Paper presented at the annual conference of the Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, San Antonio, Texas.
Nuffield, J. (1982). Parole decision making in Canada: Research towards decisions guidelines. Ottawa, Canada: Department of the Solicitor General of Canada.
*Nunes, K. L., Serran, G. A., Firestone, P., Greenberg, D. M., & Bradford, J. M. (2000, November). A comparison of child molesters who deny vs. those who admit their offenses. Paper presented at the annual conference for the Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, San Diego, CA.
*Nunes, K. L., Firestone, P., Bradford, J. M., Greenberg, D. M. & Broom, I. (2002). A comparison of modified versions of the Static-99 and the Sex Offender Risk Appraisal Guide (SORAG). Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 14(3), 253-269.
*Nutbrown, V., & Stasiak, E. (1987). A retrospective analysis of O.C.I. cost effectiveness 1977-81 (Ontario Correctional Institute Research Monograph No. 2). Brampton, Ontario, Canada: Ontario Ministry of Correctional Services.
*Ohio Department of Rehabilitation and Correction. (2001). Ten-year recidivism follow-up of 1989 sex offender releases. OH: Author.
*Pacht, A. R., & Roberts, L. M. (1968). Factors related to parole experience and the deviated sex offender. Correctional Psychologist, 3, 8-11.
*Perkins, D. (1987). A psychological treatment programme for sex offenders. In B. J. McGurk, D. M. Thornton, and M. Williams (Eds.), Applying psychology to imprisonment: Theory and practice (pp. 191-249). London: Her Majesty's Stationery Office.
*Poole, D., Liedecke, D., & Marbibi, M. (2000). Risk assessment and recidivism in juvenile sexual offenders: A validation study of the Static-99. TX: Texas Youth Commission.
*Prentky, R., Harris, B., Frizzell, K., & Righthand, S. (2000). An actuarial procedure for assessing risk with juvenile sex offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 12, 71-93.
*Prentky, R., Knight, R. A., & Lee, A. F. S. (1997). Risk factors associated with recidivism among extrafamilial child molesters. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 141-149.
*Prentky, R. A., Knight, R.A., Lee, A. F., & Cerce, D.D. (1995). Predictive validity of lifestyle impulsivity for rapists. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 22, 106-128.
*Proulx, J., Pellerin, B., McKibben, A., Aubut, J., & Ouimet, M. (1995). [Static and dynamic predictors in sexual aggressors].Unpublished raw data.
*Quinsey, V. L., Khanna, A., & Malcolm, P. B. (1998). A retrospective evaluation of the Regional Treatment Centre sex offender treatment program. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13, 621-644.
Quinsey, V. L., Lalumière, M. L., Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1995). Predicting sexual offenses. In J. C. Campbell (Ed.), Assessing dangerousness: Violence by sexual offenders, batterers, and child abusers (pp. 114-137). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
*Quinsey,V. L., Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1995). Actuarial prediction of sexual recidivism. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 85-105.
Quinsey, V. L., Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., & Cormier, C. A. (1998). Violent offenders: appraising and managing risk. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
*Rabinowitz-Greenberg, S. R. (1999). Predictors of recidivism in a population of Canadian exhibitionists: Psychological, phallometric and offense factors. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Canada.
*Rabinowitz-Greenberg, S. R., Firestone, P., Bradford, J. M., & Greenberg, D. M. (2002). Prediction of recidivism in exhibitionists: Psychological, phallometric, and offence factors. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 14 (4), 329-347.
*Radzinowicz, L. (1957). Sexual offenses: A report of the Cambridge Department of Criminal Science. London: MacMillan & Co. Ltd.
*Rasmussen, L. A. (1999). Factors related to recidivism among juvenile sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 11, 69-85.
Raymond, N. C., Coleman, E., Ohlerking, R., Christensen, G. A., & Miner, M. (1999). Psychiatric comorbidity in pedophilic sex offenders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 786-788.
*Reddon, J. R., Studer, L., & Estrada, L. (1996). [Recidivism data from the Pheonix Program for sex offender treatment]. Unpublished raw data.
*Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1995). Cross-validation and extension of an actuarial instrument for the prediction of recidivism among sex offenders (Penetanguishene Mental Health Centre Research Report, Vol. XII (2)). Penetanguishene, Ontario, Canada.
*Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1997). Cross-validation and extension of the Violence Risk Appraisal Guide for child molesters and rapists. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 231-241.
*Rice, M. E., Harris, G. T., & Quinsey, V. L. (1990). A follow-up of rapists assessed in a maximum security psychiatric facility. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 5, 435-448.
*Rice, M. E., Quinsey, V. L., & Harris, G. T. (1989). Predicting sexual recidivism among treated and untreated extrafamilial child molesters released from a maximum security psychiatric institution (Penetanguishene Mental Health Centre Research Report, Vol. VI (3)). Penetanguishene, Ontario, Canada.
*Rice, M. E., Quinsey, V. L., & Harris, G. T. (1991). Sexual recidivism among child molesters released from a maximum security psychiatric institution. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 381-386.
Roberts, C. F., Doren, D. M., & Thornton, D. (2002). Dimensions associated with assessments of sex offender recidivism risk. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 29, 569-589.
*Rooth, F. G., & Marks, I. M. (1974). Persistent exhibitionism: Short-term response to aversion, self-regulation, and relaxation treatments. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 3, 227-249.
Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (rev. ed.). Newbury Park: Sage.
*Ryan, G., & Miyoshi, T. (1990). Summary of a pilot follow-up study of adolescent sexual perpetrators after treatment. Interchange, 1, 6-8.
*Santman, J. (1998). A taxonomic model of juvenile sexual offender recidivism. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology, Fresno, California.
*Schiller, G. (2000). [Hospital and aftercare clinician's estimates of recidivism probability compared with actual reoffense rates by type of outcome and type of prediction]. Unpublished raw data.
*Schram, D. D., & Milloy, C. D. (1995). Community notification: A study of offender characteristics and recidivism. Olympia, WA: Washington State Institute for Public Policy.
*Schram, D. D., Milloy, C. D., & Rowe, W. E. (1991). Juvenile sex offenders: A follow-up study of reoffense behavior. Olympia, WA: Washington State Institute for Public Policy.
Seidman, B. T., Marshall, W. L., Hudson, S. M., & Robertson, P. J. (1994). An examination of intimacy and loneliness in sex offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 518-534.
*Serin, R. C., Mailloux, D. L., & Malcolm, P. B. (2001). Psychopathy, deviant sexual arousal and recidivism among sexual offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16, 234-246.
*Sjöstedt, G., & Långström, N. (2001). Actuarial assessment of sex offender recidivism risk: A cross-validation of the RRASOR and the Static-99 in Sweden. Law and Human Behaviour, 25, 629-645.
*Sjöstedt, G., & Långström, N. (2002). Assessment of risk for criminal recidivism among rapists: A comparison of four different measures. Psychology, Crime & the Law, 8, 25-40.
Slavin, R. E. (1995). Best evidence synthesis: An intelligent alternative to meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 48, 9-18.
Smallbone, S. W., & Dadds, M. R. (1998). Childhood attachment and adult attachment in incarcerated adult male sex offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13, 555-573.
*Smiley, W. C., McHattie, L., Hills, A., & MacKenzie, K. R. (1999, May). Predicting sexual recidivism: A comparison of the RRASOR and the PCL-R. Poster presentation at the annual conference of the Canadian Psychological Association, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada.
*Smiley, W. C., McHattie, L., & Mulloy, R. (1998, October). Predicting sexual recidivism using a brief actuarial risk scale. Poster presentation at the annual conference of the Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, Vancouver, BC, Canada.
*Smith, P., & Waterman, M. (in press). Processing bias for sexual material: The emotional stroop and sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment.
*Smith, W. R., & Monastersky, C. (1986). Assessing juvenile sexual offenders' risk for reoffending. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 13, 115-140.
*Song, L., & Lieb, R. (1994). [Recidivism data for the study of Washington State's sentencing alternative for sex offenders].Unpublished raw data.
*Soothill, K. L., Jack, A., & Gibbens, T. C. N. (1976). Rape: A 22-year cohort study. Medicine, Science, and the Law, 16, 62-69.
*Soothill, K. L., Way, C. K., & Gibbens, T. C. N. (1980). Rape acquittals. Modern Law Review, 43, 159-172.
*Sturgeon, V. H. & Taylor, J. (1980). Report of a five-year follow-up study of mentally disordered sex offenders released from Atascadero State Hospital in 1973. Criminal Justice Journal, 4, 31-63.
*Sturup, G. K. (1953). Sexual offenders and their treatment in Denmark and the other Scandinavian countries. International Review of Criminal Policy, 4, 1-19.
*Sturup, G. K. (1960). Sex offenses: The Scandinavian experience. Law and Contemporary Problems, 25, 361-375.
Swets, J. A. (1986). Indices of discrimination and diagnostic accuracy: Their ROCs and implied models. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 100-117.
Swets, J. A., Dawes, R. M., & Monahan, J. (2000). Psychological science can improve diagnostic decisions. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 1,1-26.
*Thornton, D. (1997). [A 16-year follow-up of 563 sexual offenders from HM Prison Service in 1979]. Unpublished raw data.
*Thornton, D. (2000). [Recidivism information for 110 sexual offenders released from HM Prison Service during the 1990s]. Unpublished raw data.
*Thornton, D. (2002a). Constructing and testing a framework for dynamic risk assessment. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 14(2), 139-153.
*Thornton, D. (2002b). [Factors associated with sexual recidivism in a sample of treated sex offenders]. Unpublished raw data.
*Tough, S. E. (2001). Validation of two standardized risk assessments (RRASOR, 1997; Static-99, 1999) on a sample of adult males who are developmentally disabled with significant cognitive deficits. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
*Tracy, F., Donnelly, H., Morgenbesser, L., & MacDonald, D. (1983). Program evaluation: Recidivism research involving sex offenders. In J. G. Greer & I. R. Stuart (Eds.), The sexual aggressor: Current perspectives on treatment (pp. 198-213). NY: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
*Waite, D., Pinkerton, R., Wieckowski, E., McGarvey, E., & Brown, G. L. (2002, October). Tracking treatment outcome among juvenile sexual offenders: A nine year follow-up study. Paper presented at the 21st annual conference of the Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, Montreal, Quebec.
Ward, T., Hudson, S. M., Marshall, W. L., & Siegert, R. (1995). Attachment style and intimacy deficits in sex offenders: A theoretical framework. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 7, 317-335.
Ward, T., & Siegert, R. J. (2002). Toward a comprehensive theory of child sexual abuse: A theory knitting perspective. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 8, 319-351.
*Weaver, C., & Fox, C. (1984). The Berkeley Sex Offenders Group: A seven-year evaluation. Probation Journal, 31, 143-146.
Webster, C. D., Harris, G.T., Rice, M.E., Cormier, C., & Quinsey, V. L. (1994). The Violence Prediction Scheme. Toronto, Ontario: Centre for Criminology, University of Toronto.
West, D. J. (1983). Sex offenses and offending. In M. Tonry & N. Morris (Eds.). Crime and justice: An annual review of research (pp. 183-233). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
*Williams, S. M., & Nicholaichuk, T. (2001, November). Assessing static risk factors in adult female sex offenders under federal jurisdiction. Paper presented at the annual conference of the Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, San Antonio, TX.
Wilson, R. J. (1999). Emotional congruence in sexual offenders against children. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 11, 33-47.
*Wilson, R. J., & Prinzo, M. (2001, November). The concurrent validity of actuarial measures of sexual and violent risk in high-risk sexual offenders detained until sentence completion. Paper presented at the annual conference of the Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, San Antonio, TX.
*Wing, S. W. (1984). A study of reoffenses by treated sex offenders at Western State Hospital. Unpublished manuscript.
*Witte, T., Di Placido, C., Gu, D., & Wong, S. (2002, May). Criminal Sentiments Scale: Predictive validity in a sample of sexual offenders primarily composed of rapists. Poster session presented at the Canadian Psychological Association's 63rd Annual Convention, Vancouver, BC, Canada.
*Witte, T., Di Placido, C., & Wong, S. (2001). How dangerous are dangerous sex offenders? An estimation of recidivism and level of risk using a matched control group. Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada: Regional Psychiatric Centre.
*Worling, J. R. (2001). Personality-based typology of adolescent male sexual offenders: Differences in recidivism rates, victim-selection characteristics, and personal victimization histories. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 13, 149-166.
*Worling, J. R., & Curwen, T. (2000). Adolescent sexual offender recidivism: Success of specialized treatment and implications for risk prediction. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, 965-982.
*Wormith, J. S., & Ruhl, M. (1986). Preventive detention in Canada. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 1, 399-430.
Table 1
Variable | Median | Mean | 95% C.I. | Q | k | Total | Studies | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sexual Deviancy |
||||||||
Any deviant sexual interest |
.36 |
.31 |
.21 |
.42 |
21.91 |
16 |
2,769 |
5, 6, 19, 31, 32, 43.2, 59.2, 63, 74.3, 76, 81, 82, 84, 86, 89, 91.1 |
Sexual interests in children |
.34 |
.33 |
.10 |
.57 |
2.82 |
4 |
438 |
43.2, 63, 82, 91.1 |
Paraphilic interests |
.32 |
.21 |
.01 |
.41 |
6.71 |
4 |
477 |
32, 63, 89, 91.1 |
Sexual preoccupations |
.51 |
.39 |
.23 |
.56 |
8.31 |
6 |
1,119 |
14, 27, 42.2, 59.2, 62m, 67 |
MMPI 5 – Masculinity – Femininity |
.46 |
.42 |
.16 |
.68 |
0.86 |
4 |
617 |
22, 27, 36, 47 |
Phallometric Assessment |
||||||||
Any deviant sexual preference |
.40 |
.24 |
.12 |
.35 |
12.65 |
13 |
2,180 |
2, 27, 35, 43.3, 44, 48.2, 56.2, 57, 57.1, 62m, 71, 73.1, 81 |
Sexual interest in rape/violence |
.33 |
.12 |
-.06 |
.29 |
3.96 |
7 |
1,140 |
27, 35, 43.1, 48, 62m, 71, 73.1 |
Sexual interest in children |
.37 |
.32 |
.16 |
.47 |
11.52 |
10 |
1,278 |
27, 35, 42.1, 43.2, 48, 57, 57.1, 62m, 71, 73.1 |
Sexual interest in boys |
.30 |
.20 |
-.10 |
.51 |
1.26 |
3 |
306 |
27, 35, 42.1 |
Antisocial Orientation Antisocial Personality |
||||||||
Antisocial personality disorder |
.29 |
.21 |
.11 |
.31 |
13.01 |
12 |
3,267 |
20, 21, 22, 27, 35, 36, 42.2, 47, 56.1, 59, 81, 82 |
PCL-R |
.25 |
.29 |
.20 |
.38 |
14.36 |
13 |
2,783 |
43.3, 44, 48.2, 56.3, 62m, 64, 65, 68.2, 74.1, 80, 81, 84, 89.1 |
MMPI 4 – Psychopathic deviate |
.46 |
.43 |
.18 |
.69 |
0.35 |
4 |
617 |
22, 27, 36, 47 |
Any personality disorder |
.33 |
.36 |
.18 |
.53 |
8.85 |
5 |
770 |
9, 35, 43.3, 44, 82 |
With outlier |
.36 |
.66 |
.52 |
.80 |
45.32*** |
6 |
1,985 |
66.1 |
Antisocial Traits |
||||||||
General self-regulation problems |
.34 |
.37 |
.26 |
.48 |
22.85 |
15 |
2,411 |
13, 19, 42.2, 47, 56.3, 59.1, 62m, 63, 65, 73.1, 74.1, 80, 82, 84, 91.1 |
Impulsivity, recklessness |
.25 |
.25 |
.06 |
.43 |
5.35 |
6 |
775 |
13, 42.2, 58, 59.2, 63, 91.1 |
Poor problem solving |
.37 |
.14 |
-.09 |
.37 |
3.53 |
3 |
475 |
42.2, 59.2, 74.3 |
Employment instability |
.15 |
.22 |
.13 |
.30 |
20.88 |
15 |
5,357 |
1, 8, 18, 19, 27, 43.3, 50, 52, 54, 64, 74.3, 78, 86, 88, 89 |
Antisocial traits continued MMPI 9 – Hypomania |
.08 |
.16 |
-.09 |
.42 |
9.33* |
4 |
617 |
22, 27, 36, 47 |
Any substance abuse |
.12 |
.12 |
.05 |
.18 |
52.22** |
31 |
9,166 |
5, 6, 8, 9, 18, 19, 21, 27, 29, 35, 36, 37, 42.2, 44, 45, 46, 47, 50, 55, 59.2, 62m, 63, 64, 66.1, 74.3, 78, 81, 82, 86, 89, 90 |
Intoxicated during offence |
.04 |
.11 |
.02 |
.20 |
19.73* |
10 |
5,276 |
8, 18, 22, 27, 29, 42.2, 50, 62.9, 88, 90 |
Hostility |
.16 |
.17 |
.04 |
.31 |
12.69 |
9 |
1,960 |
22, 27, 35, 36, 42.2, 59, 62m, 73.1, 76 |
Procriminal attitudes |
.42 |
.18 |
-.05 |
.40 |
2.85 |
3 |
393 |
59.2, 68.1, 81 |
History of Rule Violation |
||||||||
Childhood behaviour problems |
.17 |
.30 |
.16 |
.43 |
7.11 |
8 |
1,996 |
5, 6, 29, 44, 59.2, 64, 81, 90 |
Childhood criminality |
.32 |
.24 |
.12 |
.37 |
12.91 |
16 |
2,849 |
5, 6, 8, 11, 14, 29, 34, 42.2, 45, 50, 59, 63, 64.1, 67, 81, 87 |
Any prior criminal history |
.30 |
.32 |
.27 |
.37 |
76.56*** |
31 |
14,800 |
8, 9, 18, 19, 22, 24.1, 27, 30.2, 31, 35, 36, 39, 41.1, 42.2, 43.3, 44, 50, 51, 54, 55, 62m, 63, 66, 71, 74.3, 77, 78, 83, 89, 90 |
History of non-sexual crime |
.29 |
.26 |
.18 |
.34 |
13.87 |
12 |
8,151 |
8, 12.2, 20, 30.2, 35, 36, 43.3, 50, 52, 59.2, 86, 92 |
History of violent crime |
.19 |
.18 |
.12 |
.25 |
43.21** |
19 |
7,448 |
8, 18, 19, 22, 31, 35, 39, 42.2, 43.3, 44, 52, 59.2, 62m, 64.1, 66, 71, 74.3, 83, 90 |
History of non-violent crime |
.11 |
.16 |
.08 |
.25 |
21.34* |
10 |
3,706 |
6, 8, 22, 35, 39, 43.3, 44, 74.3, 83, 90 |
With outlier |
.10 |
.09 |
.01 |
.17 |
48.57*** |
11 |
4,084 |
81 |
Non-compliance with supervision |
.73 |
.62 |
.45 |
.79 |
5.86 |
3 |
2,159 |
78, 86, 88 |
Violation of conditional release |
.44 |
.50 |
.34 |
.65 |
16.55*** |
4 |
2,151 |
39, 44, 74.3, 90 |
Adverse Childhood Environment |
||||||||
Separation from parents |
.16 |
.16 |
.05 |
.28 |
11.74 |
13 |
4,145 |
18, 29, 34, 42.2, 43.3, 44, 56.1, 59.2, 62m, 71, 76, 90, 92 |
Childhood neglect, physical or emotional abuse |
.00 |
.10 |
.01 |
.20 |
27.43 |
18 |
5,490 |
5, 6, 8, 18, 27, 29, 42.2, 50, 56.1, 59, 62m, 71, 74.3, 78, 81, 87, 90, 92 |
Childhood sexual abuse |
.02 |
.09 |
-.01 |
.18 |
24.44 |
17 |
5,711 |
6, 8, 18, 22, 27, 42.2, 45, 50, 55, 59, 62m, 71, 78, 81, 90, 91.1, 92 |
Negative relation with father |
.05 |
.07 |
-.18 |
.32 |
2.53 |
4 |
572 |
22, 36, 55, 59.2 |
Negative relation with mother |
.10 |
.09 |
-.16 |
.34 |
1.37 |
4 |
565 |
22, 36, 55, 59.2 |
Intimacy Deficits |
||||||||
General people problems |
.02 |
.15 |
-.05 |
.36 |
9.72 |
9 |
860 |
14, 18, 27, 35, 47, 59, 63, 64, 67 |
Social skill deficits |
-.24 |
-.07 |
-.27 |
.13 |
8.11 |
6 |
965 |
6, 36, 42.2, 47, 59.2, 76 |
Negative social influences |
.21 |
.22 |
-.01 |
.45 |
2.36 |
6 |
938 |
18, 27, 29, 34, 59.2, 78 |
Conflicts in intimate relationship |
.42 |
.36 |
.05 |
.66 |
2.08 |
4 |
298 |
8, 35, 46, 74.3 |
Emotional identification with children |
.63 |
.42 |
.16 |
.69 |
4.32 |
3 |
419 |
20, 63, 73.1 |
Loneliness |
.02 |
.03 |
-.10 |
.17 |
5.79 |
6 |
1,810 |
5, 6, 27, 59.2, 73.1, 76 |
Sexual Attitudes |
||||||||
Attitudes tolerant of sexual crime |
.21 |
.22 |
.05 |
.38 |
14.53* |
9 |
1,617 |
1, 5, 6, 31, 59.2, 62m, 73.1, 74.3, 76 |
Child molester attitudes |
.04 |
.09 |
-.16 |
.33 |
7.67 |
4 |
832 |
1, 35, 62m, 76 |
Other deviant sex attitudes |
-.08 |
-.04 |
-.28 |
.19 |
4.60 |
3 |
683 |
5, 22, 62m |
Low sex knowledge |
.25 |
.11 |
-.06 |
.27 |
3.49 |
5 |
1,364 |
6, 27, 36, 42.2, 62m |
General Psychological Problems |
||||||||
General psychological functioning |
.13 |
.12 |
-.05 |
.30 |
5.77 |
8 |
1,403 |
18, 22, 35, 62m, 62.9, 71, 73.1, 76 |
With outlier |
.19 |
.30 |
.16 |
.44 |
16.74* |
9 |
2,618 |
66.1 |
Anxiety |
.06 |
.07 |
-.18 |
.31 |
0.57 |
6 |
667 |
22, 27, 35, 36, 76, 82 |
Depression |
-.17 |
-.13 |
-.34 |
.08 |
6.90 |
7 |
850 |
5, 22, 27, 35, 36, 59, 76 |
MMPI 2 – Depression |
.18 |
.09 |
-.17 |
.34 |
2.37 |
4 |
617 |
22, 27, 36, 47 |
Low self-esteem |
.03 |
.04 |
-.12 |
.20 |
10.12 |
10 |
1,424 |
5, 22, 27, 36, 42.2, 47, 55, 59, 73.1, 76 |
Severe psychological dysfunction |
.00 |
-.03 |
-.19 |
.12 |
0.73 |
8 |
1,268 |
5, 20, 27, 35, 44, 56.1, 59.2, 74.3 |
With outlier |
.00 |
.24 |
.11 |
.38 |
41.06** |
9 |
2,783 |
66.1 |
Seriousness of Sex Offence |
||||||||
Degree of force used |
.00 |
.09 |
.02 |
.16 |
29.28 |
25 |
7,221 |
5, 6, 8, 18, 19, 24.2, 27, 29, 36, 42.2, 43.3, 44, 50, 56.1, 57, 57.1, 59.2, 62.2, 64.1, 74.3, 81, 86, 87, 89, 90 |
Degree of sexual intrusiveness |
-.10 |
-.17 |
-.29 |
-.05 |
29.51** |
15 |
2,500 |
5, 6, 18, 19, 20, 24, 29, 30, 42.2, 50, 57.1, 62.2, 64.1, 81, 87 |
Any non-contact sexual offences |
.37 |
.31 |
.24 |
.38 |
42.07** |
22 |
10,238 |
5, 12.1, 21, 22, 24.1, 31, 33, 37, 38, 44, 50, 51, 52, 53, 54, 55, 59.2, 64.1, 66, 76, 85, 93 |
With outlier |
.37 |
.43 |
.36 |
.49 |
188.90*** |
23 |
11,612 |
90 |
Clinical Presentation |
||||||||
Lack of victim empathy |
-.01 |
-.08 |
-.21 |
.06 |
0.92 |
5 |
1,745 |
5, 6, 27, 31, 36 |
Denial of sexual crime |
-.02 |
.02 |
-.15 |
.19 |
11.72 |
9 |
1,780 |
5, 6, 29, 50, 57, 59.2, 62.6, 64.2, 74.3 |
Minimizing culpability |
.00 |
.06 |
-.18 |
.30 |
4.04 |
6 |
768 |
5, 6, 27, 29, 36, 59.2 |
Low motivation for treatment at intake |
-.04 |
-.08 |
-.21 |
.06 |
13.83 |
12 |
1,786 |
1, 5, 6, 19, 27, 28, 31, 36, 40, 49, 78, 81 |
Poor progress in treatment (post) |
.11 |
.14 |
-.03 |
.30 |
9.35 |
7 |
1,118 |
27.2, 36, 42.2, 43.2, 49, 56.4, 75 |
Approaches to Risk Assessment |
||||||||
Clinical assessment |
.34 |
.40 |
.24 |
.56 |
7.18 |
9 |
1,679 |
3, 5, 6, 23, 26, 27.1, 28, 36, 56.3 |
Empirically guided |
.31 |
.41 |
.26 |
.55 |
9.56 |
9 |
1,270 |
31, 56.3, 59.2, 65, 68.2, 73, 74m, 79, 89 |
With outlier |
.34 |
.51 |
.37 |
.65 |
26.20** |
10 |
1,391 |
84.1 |
Actuarial risk scale: sex |
.64 |
.61 |
.54 |
.69 |
70.36*** |
33 |
6,792 |
2m, 3, 7, 14, 19, 31, 43.4, 44, 50, 56.3, 58, 59.2, 62.7, 65, 66.2, 67, 68.1, 69, 70, 71, 72, 73.1, 74m, 75.1, 76, 77, 79, 80, 84.1, 85, 86.1, 89.2, 95 |
Actuarial risk scale: criminal |
.79 |
.71 |
.47 |
.94 |
4.14 |
4 |
497 |
18, 59.2, 68.1, 83 |
With outlier |
.64 |
.51 |
.33 |
.69 |
10.92* |
5 |
813 |
39 |
Unvalidated objective risk assessment scheme |
1.08 |
1.06 |
.91 |
1.19 |
12.08 |
14 |
3,093 |
1, 5, 22.1, 29, 57, 59.2, 61, 62.9, 63, 64, 75.1, 86, 89.1, 90 |
With outlier |
1.08 |
.93 |
.82 |
1.05 |
30.27** |
15 |
3,381 |
43.4 |
Risk Scales |
||||||||
VRAG |
.44 |
.52 |
.38 |
.67 |
8.05 |
5 |
1,147 |
43.5, 44, 56.3, 65, 74.1 |
SORAG |
.55 |
.48 |
.33 |
.63 |
4.21 |
5 |
1,348 |
7, 44, 56.3, 79, 86.1 |
With outlier |
.56 |
.55 |
.41 |
.69 |
16.67** |
6 |
1,421 |
74.1 |
STATIC-99 |
.70 |
.63 |
.54 |
.72 |
44.17** |
21 |
5,103 |
2.2, 3, 19, 31, 44, 50, 56.3, 59.2, 62.7, 66.2, 68.1, 69, 70, 71, 73.1, 74.2, 75.1, 79, 84.1, 86.1, 95 |
RRASOR |
.86 |
.59 |
.50 |
.67 |
55.84*** |
18 |
5,266 |
2.0, 2.1, 19, 44, 50, 56.3, 59.2, 65, 66.2, 70, 71, 74.1, 76, 77, 80, 86.1, 89.2 |
MnSOST-R |
.58 |
.66 |
.46 |
.86 |
4.76 |
4 |
813 |
56.3, 74.2, 86.1, 89.2 |
SVR-20 |
.48 |
.77 |
.58 |
.95 |
19.01** |
6 |
819 |
56.3, 65, 68.2, 74.1, 79, 84.1 |
Other sex risk scales |
.59 |
.66 |
.56 |
.76 |
13.04 |
8 |
2,751 |
19, 27.2, 52, 56.3, 58, 68.2, 86, 89.2 |
SIR |
.94 |
.77 |
.52 |
1.02 |
2.51 |
3 |
420 |
18, 68.1, 83 |
With outlier |
.79 |
.52 |
.34 |
.71 |
10.61* |
4 |
736 |
39 |
*p<05, **p<.01, ***p<.001
Note: C.I. – confidence interval, k is the number of studies.
Table 2
Variable | Median | Mean | 95% C.I. | Q | k | Total | Studies | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sexual Deviancy |
||||||||
Any deviant sexual interest |
-.15 |
-.04 |
-.25 |
.18 |
4.61 |
5 |
619 |
31, 59.2, 63, 84, 91.1 |
MMPI 5 – Masculinity - femininity |
-.22 |
-.19 |
-.43 |
.06 |
1.32 |
4 |
713 |
22, 25, 27, 36 |
Antisocial Orientation Antisocial Personality |
||||||||
Antisocial personality disorder |
.60 |
.49 |
.26 |
.72 |
5.61 |
6 |
756 |
22, 27, 35, 36, 59, 82 |
PCL-R |
.56 |
.57 |
.30 |
.84 |
1.13 |
4 |
263 |
65, 68.2, 74.1, 84 |
MMPI 4 – Psychopathic deviate |
.17 |
.24 |
-.01 |
.48 |
3.72 |
4 |
713 |
22, 25, 27, 36 |
Antisocial Traits |
||||||||
General self-regulation problems |
.68 |
.62 |
.44 |
.80 |
4.28 |
8 |
725 |
13, 59.2, 63, 65, 74.1, 82, 84, 91.1 |
Impulsivity, recklessness |
.37 |
.45 |
.20 |
.70 |
1.44 |
4 |
428 |
13, 59.2, 63, 91.1 |
Employment instability |
.37 |
.41 |
.30 |
.52 |
8.77 |
5 |
3,532 |
18, 27, 50, 52, 88 |
MMPI 9 – Hypomania |
.57 |
.47 |
.23 |
.72 |
6.99 |
4 |
713 |
22, 25, 27, 36 |
Any substance abuse |
.40 |
.47 |
.33 |
.60 |
6.42 |
7 |
2,436 |
18, 27, 35, 36, 50, 59.2, 66.1 |
With outlier |
.41 |
.42 |
.28 |
.55 |
14.48* |
8 |
2,549 |
63 |
Intoxicated during offence |
.11 |
.29 |
.11 |
.46 |
1.90 |
3 |
1,569 |
22, 27, 50 |
With outlier |
.24 |
.46 |
.30 |
.63 |
28.11*** |
4 |
1,773 |
18 |
History of Rule Violation |
||||||||
Childhood criminality |
.59 |
.43 |
.27 |
.59 |
10.26 |
6 |
1,395 |
11, 34, 50, 59, 63, 64.1 |
Any prior criminal history |
.56 |
.50 |
.41 |
.59 |
21.66* |
10 |
4,090 |
18, 22, 27, 31, 35, 36, 39, 50, 52, 63 |
History of non-sexual crime |
.68 |
.51 |
.37 |
.64 |
11.45* |
5 |
1,945 |
35, 36, 50, 52, 59.2 |
History of violent crime |
.70 |
.68 |
.59 |
.78 |
22.85** |
10 |
3,637 |
18, 22, 25, 31, 35, 39, 52, 59.2, 64.1, 66 |
History of non-violent crime |
.31 |
.32 |
.16 |
.48 |
2.79 |
4 |
1,043 |
22, 25, 35, 39 |
Adverse Childhood Environment |
||||||||
Childhood neglect, physical or emotional abuse |
.08 |
-.03 |
-.23 |
.17 |
5.06 |
4 |
1,897 |
18, 27, 50, 59 |
Childhood sexual abuse |
.02 |
-.07 |
-.28 |
.14 |
7.84 |
5 |
1,900 |
18, 22, 27, 50, 91.1 |
With outlier |
.13 |
.08 |
-.10 |
.26 |
16.22** |
6 |
2,048 |
59 |
Negative relation with father |
.17 |
.29 |
-.03 |
.61 |
2.57 |
3 |
341 |
22, 36, 59.2 |
Negative relation with mother |
-.01 |
.12 |
-.20 |
.43 |
2.32 |
3 |
348 |
22, 36, 59.2 |
Intimacy Deficits |
||||||||
General people problems |
.30 |
.21 |
-.01 |
.44 |
2.61 |
5 |
645 |
18, 27, 35, 59, 63 |
Negative social influences |
.71 |
.24 |
-.05 |
.54 |
41.78*** |
3 |
411 |
18, 27, 59.2 |
General Psychological Problems |
||||||||
General psychological functioning |
.43 |
.38 |
.21 |
.54 |
1.94 |
3 |
1,528 |
18, 35, 66.1 |
With outlier |
.29 |
.31 |
.15 |
.47 |
11.18* |
4 |
1,605 |
22 |
Anxiety |
-.42 |
-.31 |
-.65 |
.03 |
2.44 |
5 |
465 |
22, 27, 35, 36, 82 |
Depression |
-.12 |
-.11 |
-.38 |
.16 |
9.03 |
5 |
629 |
22, 27, 35, 36, 59 |
MMPI 2 – Depression |
.10 |
.10 |
-.16 |
.37 |
5.08 |
3 |
636 |
25, 27, 36 |
With outlier |
-.11 |
-.04 |
-.29 |
.20 |
12.72* |
4 |
713 |
22 |
Low self-esteem |
-.34 |
-.28 |
-.69 |
.14 |
2.34 |
3 |
288 |
22, 27, 36 |
With outlier |
-.18 |
.13 |
-.16 |
.42 |
9.56* |
4 |
415 |
59 |
Severe psychological dysfunction |
.24 |
.12 |
-.06 |
.29 |
1.09 |
4 |
1,450 |
27, 35, 59.2, 66.1 |
Seriousness of Sex Offence |
||||||||
Degree of force used |
.33 |
.35 |
.22 |
.47 |
8.37 |
6 |
2,501 |
18, 27, 36, 50, 59.2, 64.1 |
Degree of sexual intrusiveness |
.42 |
.36 |
.17 |
.55 |
1.29 |
3 |
956 |
18, 50, 64.1 |
Any non-contact sexual offences |
-.02 |
-.03 |
-.22 |
.16 |
13.57* |
7 |
1,889 |
22, 31, 50, 52, 59.2, 64.1, 85 |
Clinical Presentation |
||||||||
Lack of victim empathy |
.17 |
.19 |
.03 |
.35 |
1.42 |
3 |
1,435 |
27, 31, 36 |
Minimizing culpability |
.02 |
.03 |
-.31 |
.37 |
0.13 |
3 |
364 |
27, 36, 59.2 |
Low motivation for treatment at intake |
.24 |
.24 |
-.02 |
.50 |
0.48 |
3 |
488 |
27, 31, 36 |
Approaches to Risk Assessment |
||||||||
Clinical assessment |
.23 |
.24 |
-.09 |
.57 |
0.10 |
3 |
552 |
25, 26, 36 |
Empirically guided |
.33 |
.34 |
.16 |
.52 |
2.89 |
7 |
611 |
31, 59.2, 65, 68.2, 74m, 79, 84.1 |
Actuarial risk scale: sex |
.45 |
.44 |
.33 |
.55 |
13.91 |
10 |
2,712 |
31, 50, 59.2, 65, 66.2, 68.1, 74.2, 79, 84.1, 86.1 |
Actuarial risk scale: criminal |
.75 |
.77 |
.58 |
.96 |
0.67 |
4 |
639 |
18, 39, 59.2, 68.1 |
Risk Scales |
||||||||
SORAG |
.75 |
.77 |
.53 |
1.01 |
0.07 |
3 |
415 |
74.1, 79, 86.1 |
STATIC-99 |
.38 |
.44 |
.26 |
.54 |
8.11 |
8 |
1,440 |
31, 50, 59.2, 68.1, 74.2, 79, 84.1, 86.1 |
With outlier |
.46 |
.55 |
.44 |
.66 |
20.94* |
9 |
2,743 |
66.2 |
RRASOR |
.18 |
.22 |
.09 |
.34 |
5.63 |
6 |
2,223 |
50, 59.2, 65, 66.2, 74.1, 86.1 |
SVR-20 |
.25 |
.35 |
.12 |
.57 |
1.81 |
5 |
375 |
65, 68.2, 74.1, 79, 84.1 |
SIR |
.76 |
.77 |
.57 |
.97 |
0.66 |
3 |
562 |
18, 39, 68.1 |
*p<05, **p<.01, ***p<.001
Note: C.I. – confidence interval, k is the number of studies.
Table 3
Variable |
Median |
Mean |
95% C.I. |
Q |
k |
Total |
Studies |
|||
Sexual Deviancy |
||||||||||
Any deviant sexual interest |
.20 |
.18 |
.04 |
.32 |
5.41 |
7 |
1,076 |
19, 31, 59.2, 63, 74.3, 81, 84 |
||
Sexual preoccupations |
.30 |
.28 |
.13 |
.42 |
0.46 |
4 |
1,047 |
27, 42.2, 59.2, 62m |
||
MMPI 5 – Masculinity – Femininity |
.14 |
.16 |
-.06 |
.38 |
1.27 |
3 |
559 |
22, 27, 36 |
||
Phallometric Assessment |
||||||||||
Any deviant sexual preference |
.25 |
.19 |
.08 |
.29 |
8.21 |
8 |
1,768 |
27, 35, 43.3, 44, 48.2, 56.2, 62m, 81 |
||
Sexual interest in rape/violence |
.46 |
.15 |
.00 |
.31 |
8.49 |
5 |
1,038 |
27, 35, 43.1, 48, 62m |
||
Sexual interest in children |
.19 |
.18 |
.03 |
.34 |
0.96 |
5 |
992 |
27, 35, 43.0, 48, 62m |
||
Antisocial Orientation Antisocial Personality |
||||||||||
Antisocial personality disorder |
.38 |
.37 |
.23 |
.51 |
3.46 |
7 |
1,319 |
4, 22, 27, 35, 36, 42.2, 81 |
||
PCL-R |
.67 |
.58 |
.49 |
.67 |
9.63 |
9 |
2,446 |
4, 43.3, 44, 48.2, 56.3, 62m, 74.1, 81, 84 |
||
MMPI 4 – Psychopathic deviate |
.16 |
.28 |
.06 |
.50 |
2.61 |
3 |
559 |
22, 27, 36 |
||
Any personality disorder |
.25 |
.40 |
.23 |
.56 |
6.84** |
3 |
628 |
35, 43.3, 44 |
||
Antisocial Traits |
||||||||||
General self-regulation problems |
.99 |
.52 |
.42 |
.63 |
28.36*** |
8 |
1,810 |
19, 42.2, 56.3, 59.1, 62m, 63, 74.1, 84 |
||
Impulsivity, recklessness |
.47 |
.34 |
.15 |
.54 |
0.84 |
3 |
497 |
42.2, 59.2, 63 |
||
Poor problem solving |
.41 |
.13 |
-.06 |
.33 |
4.46 |
3 |
476 |
42.2, 59.2, 74.3 |
||
Employment instability |
.32 |
.35 |
.26 |
.43 |
6.89 |
6 |
3,705 |
18, 19, 27, 50, 52, 88 |
||
With outlier |
.33 |
.37 |
.29 |
.45 |
15.86* |
7 |
3,800 |
74.3 |
||
MMPI 9 – Hypomania |
.32 |
.44 |
.22 |
.67 |
4.49 |
3 |
559 |
22, 27, 36 |
||
Any substance abuse |
.35 |
.38 |
.30 |
.45 |
29.80** |
14 |
4,306 |
18, 19, 27, 35, 36, 42.2, 44, 50, 59.2, 62m, 63, 74.3, 81, 90 |
||
Intoxicated during offence |
.19 |
.22 |
.13 |
.31 |
7.08 |
6 |
3,463 |
22, 27, 42.2, 50, 62.9, 90 |
||
With outlier |
.27 |
.28 |
.20 |
.37 |
55.73*** |
7 |
3,667 |
18 |
||
Hostility |
.15 |
.21 |
.08 |
.34 |
5.10 |
6 |
1,494 |
22, 27, 35, 36, 42.2, 62m |
||
History of Rule Violation |
||||||||||
Childhood behaviour problems |
.43 |
.48 |
.35 |
.61 |
0.66 |
4 |
1,591 |
44, 59.2, 81, 90 |
||
Childhood criminality |
.46 |
.45 |
.31 |
.59 |
3.84 |
6 |
1,518 |
34, 42.2, 50, 59.2, 63, 81 |
||
Any prior criminal history |
.48 |
.48 |
.43 |
.53 |
41.26** |
19 |
8,946 |
18, 19, 22, 27, 31, 35, 36, 39, 41.1, 42.2, 43.3, 44, 50, 52, 62m, 63, 66, 74.3, 90 |
||
History of rule violation continued |
||||||||||
History of non-sexual crime |
.52 |
.58 |
.51 |
.66 |
30.75*** |
7 |
4,170 |
12.2, 35, 36, 43.3, 50, 52, 59.2 |
||
History of violent crime |
.49 |
.48 |
.42 |
.54 |
52.77*** |
16 |
6,807 |
18, 19, 22, 31, 35, 39, 41.3, 42.2, 43.3, 44, 52, 59.2, 62m, 66, 74.3, 90 |
||
History of non-violent crime |
.39 |
.36 |
.28 |
.44 |
86.91*** |
8 |
3,377 |
22, 35, 37, 43.3, 44, 74.3, 81, 90 |
||
Violation of conditional release |
.37 |
.42 |
.27 |
.56 |
1.39 |
3 |
2,080 |
39, 44, 90 |
||
With outlier |
.81 |
.48 |
.35 |
.62 |
10.01* |
4 |
2,175 |
74.3 |
||
Adverse Childhood Environment |
||||||||||
Separation from parents |
.21 |
.19 |
.10 |
.29 |
1.28 |
7 |
2,934 |
18, 42.2, 43.3, 44, 59.2, 62m, 90 |
||
Childhood neglect, physical or emotional abuse |
.33 |
.25 |
.16 |
.35 |
26.42*** |
9 |
4,184 |
18, 27, 42.2, 50, 59.2, 62m, 74.3, 81, 90 |
||
Childhood sexual abuse |
.02 |
-.05 |
-.13 |
.04 |
12.32 |
10 |
6,557 |
18, 22, 27, 42.2, 50, 59.2, 62m, 81, 90, 91.1 |
||
Negative relation with father |
.14 |
.21 |
-.06 |
.47 |
0.89 |
3 |
341 |
22, 36, 59.2 |
||
Negative relation with mother |
.19 |
.19 |
-.08 |
.45 |
1.71 |
3 |
348 |
22, 36, 59.2 |
||
Intimacy Deficits |
||||||||||
General people problems |
.36 |
.31 |
.10 |
.53 |
2.88 |
5 |
621 |
18, 27, 35, 59.1, 63 |
||
Social skill deficits |
.02 |
.03 |
-.16 |
.23 |
4.62 |
3 |
561 |
36, 42.2, 59.2 |
||
Negative social influences |
.35 |
.29 |
.06 |
.52 |
7.65* |
3 |
413 |
18, 27, 59.2 |
||
Sexual Attitudes |
||||||||||
Attitudes tolerant of sexual crime |
.17 |
.14 |
-.03 |
.31 |
3.56 |
4 |
849 |
31, 59.2, 62m, 74.3 |
||
Low sex knowledge |
.15 |
.10 |
-.05 |
.24 |
3.08 |
4 |
1,144 |
27, 36, 42.2, 62m |
||
General Psychological Problems |
||||||||||
General psychological functioning |
.05 |
.07 |
-.09 |
.23 |
1.58 |
5 |
1,004 |
18, 22, 35, 62m, 62.9 |
||
Anxiety |
-.13 |
-.07 |
-.35 |
.16 |
1.19 |
4 |
405 |
22, 27, 35, 36 |
||
Depression |
.28 |
.07 |
-.17 |
.31 |
8.93* |
4 |
497 |
27, 35, 36, 59.2 |
||
Low self-esteem |
-.14 |
-.06 |
-.23 |
.12 |
0.60 |
5 |
671 |
22, 27, 36, 42.2, 59.2 |
||
Severe psychological dysfunction |
.02 |
-.06 |
-.21 |
.10 |
2.66 |
5 |
726 |
27, 35, 44, 59.2, 74.3 |
||
Seriousness of Sex Offence |
||||||||||
Degree of force used |
.23 |
.22 |
.15 |
.29 |
9.21 |
12 |
4,920 |
18, 19, 27, 36, 42.2, 44, 50, 59.2, 62.2, 74.3, 81, 90 |
||
Degree of sexual intrusiveness |
.08 |
.05 |
-.07 |
.18 |
8.06 |
6 |
1,488 |
18, 19, 42.2, 50, 62.2, 81 |
||
Any non-contact sexual offences |
.02 |
.11 |
.04 |
.18 |
33.08*** |
9 |
7,141 |
12.1, 22, 31, 44, 50, 52, 59.2, 66, 90 |
||
Clinical Presentation |
||||||||||
Lack of victim empathy |
.01 |
.03 |
-.10 |
.15 |
0.80 |
3 |
1,435 |
27, 31, 36 |
||
Denial of sexual crime |
.10 |
.13 |
-.03 |
.29 |
8.93 |
5 |
1,294 |
3, 50, 59.2, 62m, 74.3 |
||
Minimizing culpability |
.04 |
.02 |
-.26 |
.29 |
2.90 |
3 |
364 |
27, 36, 59.2 |
||
Low motivation for treatment at intake |
.08 |
.11 |
-.04 |
.26 |
5.79 |
5 |
963 |
19, 27, 31, 36, 81 |
||
Poor progress in treatment (post) |
.03 |
.02 |
-.14 |
.19 |
0.29 |
3 |
719 |
36, 42.2, 56.4 |
||
Approaches to Risk Assessment |
||||||||||
Clinical assessment |
.53 |
.58 |
.36 |
.80 |
8.25* |
4 |
956 |
3, 16, 26, 36 |
||
With outlier |
.31 |
.33 |
.17 |
.48 |
18.19** |
5 |
1,363 |
56.3 |
||
Empirically guided |
.53 |
.52 |
.30 |
.75 |
6.85* |
3 |
373 |
31, 59.2, 74.1 |
||
With outlier |
.40 |
.31 |
.15 |
.47 |
13.52** |
4 |
780 |
56.3 |
||
Actuarial risk scale: sex |
.64 |
.58 |
.50 |
.65 |
53.83*** |
15 |
4,807 |
2m, 3, 19, 31, 43.5, 44, 50, 56.3, 59.2, 62.7, 66, 70, 74.1, 84.1, 86.1 |
||
Actuarial risk scale: criminal |
.73 |
.79 |
.60 |
.97 |
1.42 |
3 |
567 |
18, 39, 59.2 |
||
Unvalidated objective risk assessment scheme |
.79 |
.77 |
.58 |
.95 |
0.54 |
4 |
1,220 |
43.4, 62.9, 63, 90 |
||
Risk Scales |
||||||||||
VRAG |
.87 |
.80 |
.66 |
.93 |
5.42 |
3 |
1,023 |
43.5, 44, 56.3 |
||
With outlier |
.91 |
.85 |
.72 |
.98 |
12.68** |
4 |
1,118 |
74 |
||
SORAG |
.72 |
.75 |
.62 |
.88 |
3.21 |
4 |
1,308 |
44, 56.3, 62.7, 86.1 |
||
With outlier |
.74 |
.81 |
.69 |
.94 |
17.47** |
5 |
1,403 |
74.1 |
||
STATIC-99 |
.51 |
.57 |
.49 |
.64 |
48.01*** |
13 |
4,428 |
2.2, 3, 19, 31, 44, 50, 56.3, 59.2, 62.7, 66, 70, 84.1, 86.1 |
||
RRASOR |
.28 |
.34 |
.26 |
.42 |
38.16** |
10 |
3,603 |
2.2, 19, 44, 50, 56.3, 59.2, 66, 70, 74.1, 86.1 |
||
Other sex risk scales |
.58 |
.55 |
.44 |
.66 |
3.24 |
4 |
1,820 |
19, 52, 56.3, 86 |
*p<05, **p<.01, ***p<.001
Note: C.I. – confidence interval, k is the number of studies.
Table 4
Variable | Median | Mean | 95% C.I. | Q | k | Total | Studies | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sexual Deviancy |
||||||||
Any deviant sexual interest |
.03 |
-.10 |
-.20 |
.01 |
34.79*** |
8 |
2,207 |
5, 6, 31, 59.2, 62.1, 81, 84, 86 |
With outlier |
.05 |
.19 |
.11 |
.28 |
193.68*** |
9 |
5,392 |
30 |
Sexual preoccupations |
.33 |
.37 |
.23 |
.51 |
5.19 |
5 |
875 |
14, 27, 59.2, 62m, 67 |
MMPI 5 – Masculinity – Femininity |
-.04 |
-.01 |
-.19 |
.16 |
1.31 |
4 |
617 |
22, 27, 36, 47 |
Phallometric Assessment |
||||||||
Any deviant sexual preference |
.17 |
.26 |
.14 |
.38 |
9.82 |
7 |
1,263 |
27, 35, 42.1, 42.3, 48.2, 62m, 81 |
With outlier |
.19 |
.17 |
.06 |
.28 |
24.75*** |
8 |
1,478 |
56.2 |
Sexual interest in rape/violence |
.18 |
.18 |
.06 |
.31 |
1.64 |
5 |
1,075 |
27, 35, 43.1, 48, 62m |
Sexual interest in children |
.27 |
.23 |
.10 |
.36 |
4.37 |
5 |
984 |
27, 35, 42.1, 48, 62m |
Sexual interest in boys |
.20 |
.15 |
-.09 |
.38 |
3.16 |
3 |
309 |
27, 35, 42.1 |
Antisocial Orientation Antisocial Personality |
||||||||
Antisocial personality disorder |
.52 |
.55 |
.44 |
.66 |
13.03 |
11 |
4,770 |
4, 20, 22, 27, 30, 35, 36, 47, 56.1, 80, 81 |
PCL-R |
.71 |
.67 |
.57 |
.76 |
9.04 |
9 |
1,966 |
4, 15, 42.3, 48.2, 56.3, 62m, 64, 81, 84 |
MMPI 4 – Psychopathic deviate |
.32 |
.28 |
.10 |
.45 |
5.13 |
4 |
617 |
22, 27, 36, 47 |
Any personality disorder |
.44 |
.50 |
.29 |
.71 |
5.73 |
5 |
927 |
9, 35, 43.0, 62.1, 80 |
Antisocial Traits |
||||||||
General self-regulation problems |
.75 |
.75 |
.63 |
.86 |
7.56 |
6 |
1,300 |
13, 42.3, 56.3, 59.1, 62m, 84 |
With outlier |
.70 |
.72 |
.60 |
.83 |
15.34* |
7 |
1,350 |
47 |
Employment instability |
.34 |
.33 |
.27 |
.39 |
16.89 |
11 |
5,402 |
8, 12, 18, 27, 50, 52, 56, 62.1, 64, 86, 88 |
MMPI 9 – Hypomania |
.56 |
.47 |
.28 |
.66 |
5.50 |
3 |
559 |
22, 27, 36 |
With outlier |
.26 |
.40 |
.22 |
.58 |
13.81* |
4 |
617 |
47 |
Any substance abuse |
.30 |
.44 |
.39 |
.49 |
76.94*** |
21 |
9,528 |
5, 6, 8, 9, 18, 27, 29, 30, 35, 36, 42, 47, 50, 56, 59.2, 62m, 64, 81, 86, 88, 90 |
Intoxicated during offence |
.21 |
.38 |
.32 |
.43 |
48.01*** |
10 |
5,838 |
8, 17, 18, 22, 27, 29, 50, 62m, 88, 90 |
Hostility |
.30 |
.31 |
.19 |
.43 |
3.99 |
5 |
1,251 |
22, 27, 35, 36, 62m |
History of Rule Violation |
||||||||
Childhood behaviour problems |
.58 |
.56 |
.44 |
.68 |
7.12 |
7 |
1,736 |
6, 29, 56, 59.2, 64, 81, 90 |
With outlier |
.55 |
.50 |
.39 |
.62 |
14.98* |
8 |
1,848 |
5 |
Childhood criminality |
.45 |
.46 |
.35 |
.58 |
8.29 |
12 |
2,053 |
5, 6, 8, 11, 14, 29, 34, 50, 55, 59.2, 64, 81 |
With outliers |
.42 |
.34 |
.24 |
.44 |
24.31* |
14 |
2,571 |
17, 67 |
Any prior criminal history |
.55 |
.55 |
.51 |
.60 |
49.88*** |
20 |
11,251 |
8, 9, 17, 18, 22, 27, 30.2, 31, 35, 36, 39, 41m, 43.0, 50, 52, 62, 77, 80, 90, 94 |
History of non-sexual crime |
.61 |
.63 |
.57 |
.69 |
19.53* |
9 |
7,665 |
8, 12.2, 30.2, 35, 36, 50, 52, 59.2, 86 |
History of violent crime |
.56 |
.52 |
.46 |
.59 |
19.07 |
12 |
4,382 |
8, 18, 22, 31, 35, 39, 43.1, 52, 59.2, 62m, 64, 90 |
History of non-violent crime |
.57 |
.68 |
.61 |
.76 |
9.33 |
8 |
3,461 |
6, 8, 35, 39, 62.1, 80, 81, 90 |
With outlier |
.50 |
.65 |
.58 |
.72 |
19.87* |
9 |
3,652 |
22 |
Violation of conditional release |
.78 |
.74 |
.54 |
.94 |
4.80 |
3 |
1,751 |
39, 80, 90 |
Adverse Childhood Environment |
||||||||
Separation from parents |
.28 |
.27 |
.17 |
.36 |
5.83 |
6 |
2,513 |
18, 29, 56.1, 59.2, 62m, 90 |
Childhood neglect, physical or emotional abuse |
.05 |
.14 |
.06 |
.22 |
22.78* |
13 |
4,698 |
5, 6, 8, 18, 27, 29, 48.1, 50, 56.1, 59.2, 62m, 81, 96 |
Childhood sexual abuse |
-.02 |
-.03 |
-.10 |
.05 |
27.35** |
11 |
4,463 |
6, 8, 18, 22, 27, 48.1, 50, 59.2, 62m, 81, 90 |
Negative relation with father |
.09 |
.08 |
-.10 |
.27 |
2.48 |
4 |
530 |
22, 36, 48.1, 59.2 |
Negative relation with mother |
.22 |
.22 |
.04 |
.40 |
0.92 |
4 |
537 |
22, 36, 48.1, 59.2 |
Intimacy Deficits |
||||||||
General people problems |
.19 |
.17 |
.00 |
.33 |
6.18 |
8 |
723 |
14, 18, 27, 35, 47, 59.1, 64, 67 |
Social skill deficits |
-.24 |
-.09 |
-.36 |
.18 |
3.47 |
3 |
307 |
36, 47, 59.2 |
With outlier |
-.28 |
-.26 |
-.48 |
-.04 |
8.35* |
4 |
504 |
6 |
Negative social influences |
.23 |
.24 |
.06 |
.42 |
7.61 |
5 |
653 |
18, 27, 29, 55, 59.2 |
Loneliness |
-.10 |
.00 |
-.11 |
.11 |
2.68 |
4 |
1,493 |
5, 6, 27, 59.2 |
Sexual Attitudes |
||||||||
Attitudes tolerant of sexual crime |
.32 |
.29 |
.12 |
.47 |
2.96 |
5 |
617 |
5, 6, 31, 59.2, 62m |
Other deviant sex attitudes |
-.13 |
-.18 |
-.34 |
-.02 |
3.50 |
3 |
683 |
5, 22, 62m |
Low sex knowledge |
.21 |
.16 |
.03 |
.30 |
4.39 |
4 |
1,037 |
6, 27, 36, 62m |
General Psychological Problems |
||||||||
General psychological functioning |
.08 |
-.08 |
-.22 |
.07 |
0.72 |
4 |
785 |
18, 22, 35, 62m |
Anxiety |
-.03 |
-.04 |
-.26 |
.18 |
1.11 |
4 |
405 |
22, 27, 35, 36 |
Depression |
.05 |
.02 |
-.14 |
.17 |
10.57 |
7 |
1,237 |
5, 22, 27, 35, 36, 59.2, 62m |
MMPI 2 – Depression |
.02 |
-.01 |
-.19 |
.17 |
0.20 |
4 |
615 |
22, 27, 36, 47 |
Low self-esteem |
.14 |
.11 |
-.08 |
.29 |
2.77 |
6 |
529 |
5, 22, 27, 36, 47, 59.2 |
Severe psychological dysfunction |
.21 |
.12 |
-.05 |
.28 |
4.36 |
7 |
3,986 |
5, 20, 27, 30, 35, 56.1, 59.2 |
Seriousness of Sex Offence |
||||||||
Degree of force used |
.17 |
.28 |
.22 |
.33 |
33.06** |
16 |
5,940 |
5, 6, 8, 18, 27, 29, 36, 50, 56.1, 59.2, 62.1, 64, 80, 81, 86, 90 |
Degree of sexual intrusiveness |
.05 |
.18 |
.08 |
.28 |
7.76 |
9 |
1,947 |
6, 18, 24, 29, 30, 50, 62m, 64, 81 |
With outlier |
.05 |
.13 |
.03 |
.23 |
20.15* |
10 |
2,060 |
5 |
Any non-contact sexual offences |
.02 |
.07 |
.01 |
.13 |
36.01*** |
14 |
10,556 |
5, 12.1, 22, 24, 30.1, 31, 50, 52, 53, 55, 59.2, 64, 85, 90 |
Clinical Presentation |
||||||||
Lack of victim empathy |
.14 |
.12 |
.02 |
.21 |
1.26 |
5 |
1,744 |
5, 6, 27, 31, 36 |
Denial of sexual crime |
.00 |
.12 |
.02 |
.22 |
4.69 |
7 |
1,918 |
5, 6, 29, 50, 59.2, 62m, 64 |
Minimizing culpability |
.16 |
.14 |
-.02 |
.31 |
8.97 |
6 |
768 |
5, 6, 27, 29, 36, 59.2 |
Low motivation for treatment at intake |
-.14 |
-.09 |
-.21 |
.03 |
7.37 |
8 |
1,294 |
5, 6, 27, 28, 36, 40, 55, 81 |
With outlier |
-.09 |
-.01 |
-.13 |
.10 |
17.17* |
9 |
1,498 |
31 |
Poor progress in treatment (post) |
.16 |
.18 |
.03 |
.34 |
7.64 |
5 |
744 |
14, 36, 42, 56.4, 80 |
Approaches to Risk Assessment |
||||||||
Clinical assessment |
.13 |
.23 |
.09 |
.36 |
15.36* |
7 |
1,077 |
5, 6, 15, 23, 28, 36, 56.3 |
Empirically guided |
.29 |
.27 |
.14 |
.41 |
5.21 |
5 |
892 |
31, 56.3, 59.2, 79, 84.1 |
Actuarial risk scale: sex |
.58 |
.52 |
.42 |
.61 |
21.19 |
14 |
2,217 |
7, 10, 14, 31, 50, 56.3, 58, 59.2, 60, 62.7, 67, 79, 84.1, 86.1 |
Actuarial risk scale: criminal |
1.02 |
1.03 |
.84 |
1.23 |
1.19 |
3 |
532 |
10, 18, 39 |
With outlier |
.99 |
.92 |
.75 |
1.10 |
9.22* |
4 |
609 |
59.2 |
Unvalidated objective risk assessment scheme |
.94 |
.88 |
.78 |
.97 |
11.52 |
14 |
2,248 |
5, 15, 29, 43.0, 43.1, 61, 62.1, 62.1, 62.1, 62.2, 62.8, 64, 86, 91 |
With outlier |
.96 |
.93 |
.84 |
1.03 |
24.73* |
15 |
2,793 |
90 |
Risk Scales |
||||||||
SORAG |
.78 |
.79 |
.66 |
.92 |
5.36 |
5 |
1,009 |
7, 56.3, 60, 79, 86 |
STATIC-99 |
.50 |
.52 |
.42 |
.62 |
8.91 |
8 |
1,795 |
10, 31, 50, 56.3, 59.2, 79, 84.1, 86 |
RRASOR |
.25 |
.26 |
.15 |
.38 |
1.70 |
5 |
1,291 |
10, 50, 56.3, 59.2, 86 |
SVR-20 |
.51 |
.52 |
.36 |
.68 |
0.29 |
3 |
673 |
56.3, 79, 84.1 |
Other sex risk scales |
.55 |
.52 |
.41 |
.62 |
3.26 |
4 |
1,668 |
52, 56.3, 58, 86 |
*p<05, **p<.01, ***p<.001
Note: C.I. – confidence interval, k is the number of studies.
Table 5
Number | Study | Number | Study |
---|---|---|---|
1.0 |
Abel et al. (1988) |
40 |
Perkins (1987) |
1.1 |
Gore (1988) |
41.1 |
Soothill, Jack & Gibbens (1976) |
2.0 |
Haynes et al. (2000) |
41.2 |
Gibbens, Soothill & Way (1978) |
2.1 |
Nicholaichuk (1997) |
41.3 |
Soothill, Way & Gibbens (1980) |
2.2 |
Nicholaichuk (2001) |
42.0 |
Khanna, Brown, Malcolm & Williams (1989) |
3 |
Hood, Shute, Feilzer & Wilcox (2002) |
42.1 |
Malcolm, Andrews & Quinsey (1993) |
4 |
Buffington-Vollum et al. (2002) |
42.2 |
Quinsey, Khanna & Malcolm (1998) |
5 |
Smith & Monastersky (1986) |
42.3 |
Serin, Mailloux & Malcolm (2001) |
6.0 |
Kahn & Chambers (1991) |
43.0 |
Rice, Quinsey & Harris (1989) |
6.1 |
Schram, Milloy & Rowe (1991) |
43.1 |
Rice, Harris & Quinsey (1990) |
7 |
Hartwell (2001) |
43.2 |
Rice, Quinsey & Harris (1991) |
8 |
Schram & Milloy (1995) |
43.3 |
Quinsey, Rice & Harris (1995) |
9 |
Tracy et al. (1983) |
43.4 |
Rice & Harris (1995) |
10 |
Hills (2002) |
43.5 |
Rice & Harris (1997) |
11 |
Waite et al. (2002) |
44 |
Harris et al. (2003) |
12.0 |
Broadhurst & Maller (1992) |
45 |
Lab, Shields & Schondel (1993) |
12.1 |
Broadhurst & Loh (1997) |
46 |
Money & Bennet (1981) |
12.2 |
Broadhurst (1999) |
47 |
Davis, Hoffman & Stacken (1991) |
13 |
Prentky, Knight, Lee & Cerce (1995) |
48.0 |
Gretton (1995) |
14 |
Prentky et al. (2000) |
48.1 |
McBride, Gretton & Hare (1995) |
15 |
Wormith & Ruhl (1986) |
48.2 |
Gretton et al. (2001) |
16 |
Kozol, Boucher & Garofalo (1972) |
49 |
Ryan & Miyoshi (1990) |
17 |
Pacht & Roberts (1968) |
50 |
Song & Lieb (1994) |
18 |
Motiuk & Brown (1995) |
51 |
Wing (1984) |
19 |
McGrath et al. (2003) |
52 |
Thornton (1997) |
20 |
Fitch (1962) |
53 |
Radzinowicz (1957) |
21 |
Frisbie & Dondis (1965) |
54 |
Federoff et al. (1992) |
22.0 |
Hanson et al. (1993) |
55 |
Meyer & Romero (1980) |
22.1 |
Hanson et al. (1992) |
56.0 |
Barbaree, Seto & Maric (1996) |
23 |
Florida (1985) |
56.1 |
Barbaree & Seto (1998) |
24.0 |
Mair & Stevens (1994) |
56.2 |
Barbaree, Seto, Langton & Peacock (2001) |
24.1 |
Mair & Wilson (1994) |
56.3 |
Langton (2003a) |
24.2 |
Mair & Wilson (1995) |
56.4 |
Langton (2003b) |
25 |
Hall (1988) |
57.0 |
Barbaree & Marshall (1988) |
26 |
Sturgeon & Taylor (1980) |
57.1 |
Marshall & Barbaree (1988) |
27.0 |
Marques & Day (1996) |
58 |
Hanlon, Larson & Zacher (1999) |
27.1 |
Schiller (2000) |
59.0 |
Worling & Curwen (2000) |
27.2 |
Marques et al. (2002) |
59.1 |
Worling (2001) |
28 |
Dix (1976) |
59.2 |
Morton (2003) |
29 |
Santman (1998) |
60 |
Belanger & Earls (1996) |
30.0 |
Sturup (1953) |
61 |
Bench, Kramer & Erickson (1997) |
30.1 |
Sturup (1960) |
62.1 |
Bradford, Firestone, Fernandez et al. (1997) |
30.2 |
Christiansen et al. (1965) |
62.2 |
Firestone, Bradford, McCoy et al. (1998) |
31 |
Hanson (2002) |
62.3 |
Firestone, Bradford, McCoy et al. (1999) |
32 |
Rooth & Marks (1974) |
62.4 |
Firestone, Bradford, McCoy et al. (2000) |
33 |
Weaver & Fox (1984) |
62.5 |
Greenberg, Firestone, Bradford et al. (2000) |
34 |
Doshay (1943) |
62.6 |
Nunes, Serran, Firestone et al. (2000) |
35 |
Proulx et al. (1995) |
62.7 |
Nunes, Firestone, Bradford et al. (2002) |
36 |
Reddon et al. (1996) |
62.8 |
Rabinowitz-Greenberg et al. (2002) |
37 |
Meyer, Cole & Emory (1992) |
62.9 |
Rabinowitz-Greenberg (1999) |
38 |
Mohr, Turner & Jerry (1964) |
63 |
Prentky, Knight & Lee (1997) |
39 |
Bonta & Hanson (1995) |
64.0 |
Långström & Grann (2000) |
64.1 |
Långström (2002a) |
||
64.2 |
Långström (2002b) |
||
65 |
Sjöstedt & Långström (2002) |
||
66.0 |
Sjöstedt & Långström (2001) |
||
66.1 |
Långström Sjöstedt et al. (in press) |
||
66.2 |
Långström (in press) |
||
67 |
Hecker, Scoular et al. (2002) |
||
68.0 |
Witte, Di Placido & Wong (2001) |
||
68.1 |
Witte, Di Placido, Gu & Wong (2002) |
||
69 |
Poole, Liedecke & Marbibi (2000) |
||
70 |
Wilson & Prinzo (2001) |
||
71 |
Tough (2001) |
||
72 |
Williams & Nicholaichuk (2001) |
||
73.0 |
Thornton (2002a) |
||
73.1 |
Thornton (2002b) |
||
74.1 |
Dempster (1998) |
||
74.2 |
Kropp (2000) |
||
74.3 |
Dempster & Hart (2002) |
||
75.0 |
Beech, Erikson et al. (2001) |
||
75.1 |
Beech, Friendship et al. (2002) |
||
76.0 |
Hudson, Wales et al. (2002) |
||
76.1 |
Bakker, Hudson et al. (2002) |
||
77 |
Ohio (2001) |
||
78 |
Gfellner (2000) |
||
79 |
Ducro, Claix & Pham (2002) |
||
80.0 |
Mulloy & Smiley (1996) |
||
80.1 |
Mulloy & Smiley (1997) |
||
80.2 |
Smiley, McHattie & Mulloy (1998) |
||
80.3 |
Smiley, McHattie et al. (1999) |
||
81 |
Langevin & Fedoroff (2000) |
||
82 |
Berger (2002) |
||
83 |
Hanson & Harris (2001) |
||
84.0 |
Hildebrand, de Ruiter & de Vogel (in press) |
||
84.1 |
de Vogel, de Ruiter, van Beek & Mead (2002) |
||
85 |
Allam (1999) |
||
86.0 |
Fischer (2000) |
||
86.1 |
Bartosh, Garby & Lewis (2003) |
||
87 |
Cooper (2000) |
||
88 |
Minnesota Department of Corrections (1999) |
||
89.0 |
Epperson, Kaul & Huot (1995) |
||
89.1 |
Epperson, Kaul & Hesselton (1998) |
||
89.2 |
Epperson, Kaul, Huot et al.(2000) |
||
90 |
Greenberg, Da Silva & Loh (2002) |
||
91.0 |
Miner (2002a) |
||
91.1 |
Miner (2002b) |
||
92 |
Rasmussen (1999) |
||
93 |
Dwyer (1997) |
||
94 |
Nutbrown & Statiak (1987) |
||
95 |
Thornton (2000) |
Note: If a number is followed by “m”, then it indicates that the effect size was based on information from more than one article.
Appendix: Formula for calculating d
From sample sizes (N), means (M), and standard deviations (SD):
Variance of d =
Sw is the pooled within standard deviation.
Source: Cohen (1988); Hasselblad & Hedges (1995).
Not deviant | Deviant | |
---|---|---|
Recidivists |
a |
b |
Not recidivists |
c |
d |
Variance of d = , where .55133
Note: d is directly proportional to the natural logarithm of the odds ratio.
Following the advice of Fleiss (1994), .5 was added to each cell of the twofold table to allow the calculation of odds ratio in the case of empty cells.
Source: Hasselblad & Hedges (1995).
From recidivism rates when the sample size of the deviant and non-deviant groups are unknown:
N1 = sample size of recidivists
N2 = sample size of non-recidivists
Rd= recidivism rate of deviant group
Rnd = recidivism rate of non-deviant group
Variance of d =
From the definition of the odds ratio (e.g., Fleiss, 1994), and from Hasselblad & Hedges (1995).
From F/t:
Variance of d =
Note: t2 = F, for df = 1.
Source: re-arranging Formula 2.4 from Rosenthal (1991).
From r:
, or , where p is recidivism rate and q = 1- p.
Variance of d =
Source: re-arranging Formula 2.2.7 from Cohen (1988).
From c2:
, or ,
where p is recidivism rate and q = 1- p.
Variance of d =
Source: substituting definition of r (f) from Rosenthal's (1991) Formula 2.15 into
and re-arranging.
From probabilities:
For exact probabilities, find the t value that corresponds to the desired probability value with appropriate degrees of freedom. If the sample size is large (> 60), t can be approximated with Z.
, or .
Variance of d =
Source: Rosenthal (1991).
For effects that are described as “non-significant”, d = 0.
For effects that are non-significant, but a direction is given, calculate the d value needed for significance, then randomly select a two decimal number that fit between the range of zero and the minimally significant d value.
ROC areas:
Given the assumption of equal variance in the recidivists and non-recidivist groups,
, where , and Z(AUC) is the area under the ROC curve expressed as Z units.
AUC | Z(AUC) |
---|---|
.40 |
-.25 |
.50 |
.00 |
.60 |
.25 |
.75 |
.68 |
.80 |
.84 |
.90 |
1.28 |
For example, AUC = .690, Z(AUC) = .496 (from Z table), d = 1.4141(.496) = .701.
Source: Swets (1986; equation 21, p. 114).
- Date modified: